Proposal summaries

These are research proposals that have been approved by the ALSPAC exec. The titles include a B number which identifies the proposal and the date on which the proposals received ALSPAC exec approval.

Click here to export results in Word format.

B897 - Family and social influences on the development of sexual behaviour in childhood and adolescence - 26/10/2009

B number: 
B897
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Andrea Waylen (University of Bristol, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Prof Andy Ness (University of Bristol, UK), Dr Sam Leary (University of Bristol, UK), Alex Griffiths (Not used 0, Not used 0)
Title of project: 
Family and social influences on the development of sexual behaviour in childhood and adolescence.
Proposal summary: 

Romantic and sexual relationships that begin at a relatively young age are associated with an increased risk of maladaptive outcomes [1] and an adverse influence on relationship skills and sexual functioning in later life [2]. In the UK, teenage girls are at high risk of acquiring sexually transmitted infections with various pathological outcomes. More than one in four young adults in the UK now report sexual activity before their 16th birthday [3] with a mean age of sexual debut of 14 years for both boys and girls [4]. In Wales, boys and girls report a mean age of debut of 12 and 13 years respectively [5].

An "integrative model" of predictors of initiation of sexual activity in adolescence concluded that a variety of factors predicted and influenced intention to initiate sexual activity [6]. These included parental involvement in the child's life, supervision and the quality of the parent-child relationship, perceived peer norms and parental attitudes towards sex. Adolescents who experience efficient and effective parental monitoring are less likely to be involved in early sexual activity than those exposed to authoritarian or permissive monitoring [1]. There is also an association between deviant peer relationships and risky sexual activity [2] and there is evidence that risky behaviours cluster together so that individuals develop "health-compromising lifestyles" [3]. Biological factors are also associated with sexual activity with an association betweeen early puberty and early sexual debut [4] and this has been linked to family structure and the presence or otherwise of the father [5]. Adolescents whose families are in lower socio-economic groups are at increased risk of risky sexual behaviour, teenage pregnancy and also sexually transmitted infections[6, 7] However, much of the research from which these conclusions were drawn was undertaken with convenience samples and high risk or clinical groups rather than national cohorts and recommendations have been made for more research using large representative cohorts in order to improve the generalisability of results.

The aim of this study is to examine the relationship between family and social relationships in late childhood and adolescence and both early or otherwise risky sexual activity for both girls and boys in a British population birth cohort.

Methods

Study population

The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC: see www.alspac.bris.ac.uk) is a population-based study which has been described in detail elsewhere (Golding, Pembrey, & Jones, 2001). All pregnant women living in one of three Bristol-based health districts (EDD April 1991 - December 1992) were invited to take part in the study; 14,541 mothers enrolled and 13,988 infants were still alive at their first birthday.

Measurement of romantic and sexual behaviour

Romantic and sexual behaviour in the ALSPAC chort has been measured since age 11 using an adapted version of the Adolescent Sexual Activity Index [8]. This measure is administered via a computer assisted interview and asks whether romantic or sexual behaviour has been experienced, whether it was with a same- or opposite sex partner and whether drugs and / or alcohol had been used prior to the event. Each question also includes several items which are designed to assess sexual competence as described by Wellings, 2001 [9] including perceived regret and contraceptive use.

Measurement of parent-child and peer relationships

Parent-child relationships have been measured in a variety of ways across the whole span of the ALSPAC study. Some of these items (measured from infancy onwards through early and middle childhood) fit with concepts of parenting and parent-child relationship which are discussed in the literature eg measures of warmth and support, control and rejection [10] whereas others measure active aspects of the parent-child relationship such separation anxiety (infancy and childhood) and parental monitoring measured from 12 years onwards [11]. Aspects of the parent-child relationship were observed by a third-party during the completion of an interactive task between parent and child using the etch-a-sketch task at 10 and 12 years.

Potential confounders

Family and parent measures: Child age was recorded at the 9-year clinic and mother's age at the birth of the study child has also been recorded. Mother's education level was recorded in the 32-week ante-natal questionnaire and was categorised into none / Certificate of Secondary Education (CSE - a national school exam taken at 16 years), vocational, O level (a national school exam taken at 16 years but higher than CSE), A level (a national school exam taken at 18 years) or degree. Both the mother's occupation and that of her partner were also recorded at this time and these data were used to allocate the family to a social class group (classes I to V with class III split into non-manual and manual) using the 1991 Office for Population Cencuses and Surveys classification. Questions about the early sexual experience of each parent were asked in the 18- and 32-week antenatal questionnaire (mother) and in the 8-month questionnaire (father) and questions about parents' perceptions of their own childhood and the bond they shared with their own parents were asked ante-natally and at 33 months. Items about the quality of the marital relationship including the bond between parents as well as previous marriages and aggression within the relationship have been asked regularly since recrutiment into the cohort together with items about spiritual outlook and religious affiliation. Depression and anxiety have been measured regularly across the life of the cohort using both the Edinburgh post-natal depression scale and the Crown-Crisp Experiential Index. Parental locus of control was also asked at 12 years.

Child behaviour: Measures of child behaviour include the revised Rutter Behaviour Scale [12], the SDQ [13] and the DAWBA [14] - each is measured at least twice giving a choice of measures of social, behavioural and emotional development over time. The Moods and Feelings questionnaire [15] has also been adminsitered regularly from 7 years onwards. Life events have also been measured in data from the study child from 18 months onwards [16] and risky health behaviours including alcohol, tobacco and illicit substance use have been measured from 3 years onwards; anti-social behaviour was first measured at 8 years. Sensation seeking behaviour [17] has also been measured from 11 years onwards.

Biological measures: measures of puberty (Tanner staging) have been measured regularly from 97 months onwards.

Analyses

The opportunity to use data from the ALSPAC cohort to examine the influence of family and social relationships on romantic and sexual activity in adolescence means that both descriptive and inferential analyses can be undertaken to both provide an overview of relationships between all of these factors but we will also be able to start to examine causal relationships - the availability of prospective family data from infancy means that regressional analyses and structural equation modelling can be employed to determine both risk and adaptive factors which influence the development of sexual behaviour in late childhood and adolescence.

References

[1] Rai AA, Stanton B, Wu Y, Li XM, Galbraith J, Cottrell L, et al. Relative influences of perceived parental monitoring and perceived peer involvement on adolescent risk behaviors: An analysis of six cross-sectional data sets. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2003;33:108-18.

[2] Ary DV, Duncan TE, Duncan SC, Hops H. Adolescent problem behaviour: the influence of parents and peers. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1999;37:217-30.

[3] Jessor R. New Perspectives on Adolescent Risk Behvaiour. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press 1998.

[4] Brooks-Gunn J, Furstenberg Jr FF. Adolescent sexual behaviour. American Psychologist. 1989;44(2):249-57.

[5] O'Connor TG, Dunn J, Jenkins JM, Pickering K, Rasbash J. Family settings and children's adjustment: differential adjustment within and across families. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2001;179:110-5.

[6] Kotchick BA, Shaffer A, Forehand R. Adolescent sexual risk behavior: a multi-system perspective. Clinical Psychology Review. 2001;21(4):493-519.

[7] Teenage Pregnancy. Report. London: Social Exclusion Unit; 1999 June. Report No.: Cm 4342.

[8] Hansen WB, Paskett ED, Carter LJ. The adolescent sexual activity index (ASAI): a standardised strategy for measuring interpersonal heterosexual behaviours among youth. Health Education Research. 1999;14(4):485-90.

[9] Wellings K, Nanchahal K, Macdowall W, McManus S, Erens B, Mercer CH, et al. Sexual behaviour in Britain: Early heterosexual experience. Lancet (North American Edition). 2001;358(9296):1843-50.

[10] Repetti RL, Taylor SE, Seeman TE. Risky families: family social environments and the mental and physical health of offspring. Psychological Bulletin. 2002;128(2):330-66.

[11] Stattin H, Kerr M. Parental monitoring: A reinterpretation. Child Development. 2000 Jun-Aug;71(4):13.

[12] Elander J, Rutter M. Use and development of the Rutter parent's and teachers's scales. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research. 1996;6:63-78.

[13] Goodman R. Psychometric properties of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry. 2001;40(11):1337-45.

[14] Goodman R, Ford T, Richards H, Gatward R, Meltzer H. The Development and Well-being Assessment: description and initial validation of an integrated assessment of child and adolescent psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry. 2000;41:645-55.

[15] Angold A, Costello AJ, Messer SC, Pickles A, Winder F, Silver D. The development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research. 1995;5:1-12.

[16] Coddington RD. The significance of life events as etiologic factors in the diseases of children. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 1972;16:7-18.

[17] Arnett J. Sensation seeking: a new conceptualisation and a new scale. Personality and Individual Differences. 1994;16(2):289-96.

Date proposal received: 
Monday, 26 October, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Monday, 26 October, 2009
Keywords: 
Development, Sexual Behaviour
Primary keyword: 

B896 - Associations between the parent-child relationship and the health related outcomes for children - 26/10/2009

B number: 
B896
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Andrea Waylen (University of Bristol, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Title of project: 
Associations between the parent-child relationship and the health related outcomes for children.
Proposal summary: 

Various aspects of the relationship between parent and child influence the child's social and emotional development [1, 2] and there is increasing evidence that specific aspects of the parent-child relationship are also associated with general physical health both in childhood and in adulthood with more negative relationships being associated with poorer general health outcomes [3-6]. Relationships between parenting and more specific conditions such as obesity and oral health are also reported. An increased risk of obesity has been shown to be associated with parenting styles which might be considered authoritarian ie low warmth and high control or "demandingness" [7, 8] and there is some emerging work on similar associations between parenting and oral health in childhood [9-11]. There is also evidence parenting is socially patterned and that parenting changes over time as a function of changes in sociodemographic factors eg changes in the mother's health, the availability of social support and also (to a lesser extent) changes in financial circumstance; deterioration in these sociodemographic factors is associated with a reduced (less warm and supportive) parenting score over time whereas improvements in maternal health specifically are associated with an increased (more warm and supportive) parenting score. These changing relationships may in part explain some of the social patterning which can be observed in inequalities in child health.

Parent-child relationships have been classified and measured in a variety of ways but key aspects which are proving to be robust are the mother's (or main carer's) sensitivity towards and attunement to the needs of the child [12], different parenting styles [13], discipline [14] and also specific aspects of the parent-child relationship such as maternal warmth and support and hostility towards / rejection of the child [15]. As well as the provision of sustenance and stimulation, children also need their parents or carers to provide support, structure and supervision if they are to develop optimally [16]. Research has shown that parent-child relationships which are warm and supportive are associated with positive child social, emotional and physical outcomes [15, 16] whereas cold, neglecting relationships between parent and child increase the risk of problematic outcomes [3].

The aim of this proposal is to examine aspects of the parent-child relationship which have been measured over time in the ALSPAC cohort and to investigate their associations with and ability to predict child health outcomes both generally [6] and with regard to specific outcomes such as obesity and oral health. We are also interested to determine further the role of sociodemographic factors on parenting and child health outcomes. In particular we plan to investigate whether the warmth, support and control between parent and child are associated with specific conditions such as obesity and oral health but we would also like to investigate whether we can replicate a study undertaken on the NICHD Study of Early Child Care to determine the extent to which parenting mediates the detectable effects of socioeconomic risk on health in childhood [4].

Methods

Study Population: The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC: see www.alspac.bris.ac.uk) cohort is ideally placed to investigate these relationships as it has various measures of child health and also measures of the parent-child relationship and associated socio-demographic factors over time. ALSPAC is a population-based study which has been described in detail elsewhere ([17].

Measures of Health: Measures of health include maternal assessment of the child's general health (annually), number of physical symptoms in the last 12 months (annually) and also more specific measures such as DXA estimates of fat and muscle which are available at several timepoints between 9 and 17 years years. Oral health is measured via maternal report throughout childhood with questions on brushing teeth, visits to the dentist etc and via child report at 8 and 10 years. Actual observations of child dentition are also available for a subsample of the cohort in early childhood and a questionnaire about oral health is currently being administered to the cohort (age 17). Measures of the parent-child relationship include measures of attachment which have been collected between 18 months and 6 years, measures of warmth and support as well as of control, hostility and resentment (available in early childhood at eight and 33 months) and also measures of physical discipline (shouting, smacking etc) which have been collected regularly between 18 months and 6 years.

Potential confounders: A report by Waylen and Stewart-Brown [18] has shown that the sociodemographic factors associated with parenting include maternal age and education level, maternal physical and mental health (measured annually), social support and financial circumstance. Variables such as the presence of the mother's romantic partner (measured at each time point) and also ethnic group should also be accounted for.

Other possible confounders include child gender, age and birth weight / length and also gestational age; a second measure of infant weight and length collected at eight months in order to develop a measure of early rapid weight gain (models of growth have also been developed using study data); food frequency questionnaires at various ages, dietary intake of different food types including sweets and chocolate measured from 6 months onwards by maternal report and also child self-report at 10 years and diet diaries completed at 7, 10 and 13 years; the mother's occupation and that of her partner, maternal height and pre-pregnancy (used to calculate maternal BMI), maternal tobacco use both during and after pregnancy; the amount of time the child spends watching TV / playing outside or with other children (from early childhood onwards).

References

[1] Maccoby EE, Martin JA, Hetherington EM. Socialization in the context of the family: parent-child interaction. In: Anonymous, ed. Mussen Manual of Child Psychology. New York: Wiley 1983:1--102.

[2] Patterson G, DeBaryshe B, al e. A developmental perspective on antisocial behaviour. American Journal of Psychology. 1989;44:329-35.

[3] Repetti RL, Taylor SE, Seeman TE. Risky families: family social environments and the mental and physical health of offspring. Psychological Bulletin. 2002;128(2):330-66.

[4] Belsky J, Bell B, Bradley RH, Stallard N, Stewart-Brown SL. Socioeconomic risk, parenting during the preschool years and child health age 6 years. Eur J Public Health. 2007 January 12, 2007:ckl261.

[5] Stewart-Brown S, Fletcher L, Wadsworth MEJ. Parent-child relationships and health problems in adulthood in three UK national birth cohorts. European Journal of Public Health. 2005;15(6):640-6.

[6] Waylen A, Stallard N, Stewart-Brown S. Parenting and health in mid-childhood: a longitudinal study. Eur J Public Health. 2008;18(3):300-5.

[7] Rhee KE, Lumeng JC, Appugliese DP, Kaciroti N, Bradley RH. Parenting styles and overweight status in first grade. Pediatrics. 2006;117(6):2047-54.

[8] Ambrosini G, Oddy W, Robinson M, O'Sullivan T, Hands B, de Klerk N, et al. Adolescent dietary patterns are associated with lifestyle and family psycho-social factors. Public Health Nutrition. 2009;12(10):1807-15.

[9] Law CS. The impact of changing parenting styles on the advancement of pediatric oral health. J Calif Dent Assoc. 2007;35(3):192-7.

[10] Amin M, Harrison R. Understanding parents' oral health behaviors for their young children. Qualitative Health Research. 2008;19(1):116-27.

[11] Astrom A. Parental influences on adolescents' oral health behavior: two year follow-up of the Norwegian Longitudinal Health Behavior Study participants. European Journal of Dental Sciences. 1998;106:922-30.

[12] Ainsworth MDS, Bell SM, Stayton DJ, Schaffer H. Individual differences in strange-situation behaviour of one-year olds. In: Anonymous, ed. The origins of human social relations. New York: Academic Press 1971.

[13] Baumrind D. Current patterns of parental authority. Developmental Psychology. 1971;4(2):1-103.

[14] Gershoff ET. Corporal punishment by parents and associated child behaviors and experiences: A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin. 2002 Jul;128(4):539-79.

[15] Barber BK, Stolz HE, Olsen JA. Parental support, psychological control and behavioural control: assessing relevance across time, culture and method. Report. Oxford, UK: Society for Research in Child Development; 2005.

[16] Bradley R, Caldwell BM. Caregiving and the regulation of child growth and development: describing proximal aspects of the caregiving system. Developmental Review. 1995;15:38-85.

[17] Golding J, Pembrey M, Jones R. ALSPAC--the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. I. Study methodology. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2001;15(1):74-87.

[18] Waylen A, Stewart-Brown S. Parenting in ordinary families: diversity, complexity and change. York: Joseph Rowntree Foundation; 2008.

Date proposal received: 
Monday, 26 October, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Monday, 26 October, 2009
Keywords: 
Social Science
Primary keyword: 

B895 - Is there a relationship between the movies people watch and their involvement in risky or delinquent behaviour - 26/10/2009

B number: 
B895
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Andrea Waylen (University of Bristol, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Prof Andy Ness (University of Bristol, UK), Dr Sam Leary (University of Bristol, UK), Prof Marcus Munafo (University of Bristol, UK)
Title of project: 
Is there a relationship between the movies people watch and their involvement in risky or delinquent behaviour?
Proposal summary: 

One of the ways in which children learn is by modelling the behaviour of others [1]. While there is an abundance of research examining the influence of family and friends on behaviour, there is increasing evidence that exposure to risky or delinquent behaviour in the media (via video games, television programmes, films and music) is associated with increased risky and delinquent behaviours such as early sexual activity, substance use, aggressive behaviour, suicide, unhealthy eating habits and poor school performance in childhood and adolescence [2]. Given that exposure to television and movies is ubiquitous, it is important to examine associations such as these within longitudinal, prospective cohorts in order to determine exactly what the relationships are.

Research has shown that exposure to depictions of alcohol use and cigarette smoking in movies is associated with actual alcohol and tobacco use [3-5] and there is also evidence that exposure to violence in the media predicts violence and general aggression [6] and results in desensitization to real life violence [7]. Exposure to violence in the media has been reported to explain around 10% of the variance in real-life violence [2]. There is also evidence that exposure to "sexy" media is associated with precocious sexual activity in adolescence and increased likelihood of teenage pregnancy [8, 9]. However, other risk factors are also associated with delinquent behaviour in childhood and adolescence (apart from exposure to violence) including reduced academic ability and achievement, psychopathic tendencies and psychopathology / emotional disorder [6].

Unfortunately the majority of research to date has been carried out in North America which limits the generalisability of the conclusions. The aim of this proposal therefore is to examine the relationships between media exposure and a range of risky and delinquent behaviours in a UK cohort while controlling for a range of for individual, family and enviromental variables which may confound these relationships. In so doing we will compliment the work which has already been undertaken by Professor Jim Sargent from the USA.

Methods

Study Population: The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC: see www.alspac.bris.ac.uk) is a population-based study which has been described in detail elsewhere (Golding, Pembrey, & Jones, 2001). All pregnant women living in one of three Bristol-based health districts in the United Kingdom (EDD April 1991 - December 1992) were invited to take part in the study; in total 14,541 mothers enrolled and 13,988 infants were still alive at their first birthday.

Measures

Media exposure: items about television and movie exposure have been asked at various stages throughout the duration of the cohort. In childhood and early adolescence, time spent watching television and attempts to model oneself on characters (height, weight, hair colour etc) from television have been administered and in the 15+ clinic adolescents were asked which movies they had seen from a list which had been coded for risky / delinquent behaviours.

Risky / delinquent behaviours

Items asking study participants about tobacco, alcohol and substance use have been administered repeatedly since late childhood / early adolescence as have items about romantic and sexual behaviours. Although there appear to be no items relating to exposure to violence per se, questionnaires about anti-social behaviour and being the victim / perpetrator of bullying have also been asked on a regular basis. Items about eating patterns were asked in the 16+ child completed questionnaire.

Potential confounders

The comprehensive nature of the ALSPAC cohort means that a variety of potentially confounding factors are available for use in the analyses. Variables measured repeatedly over late childhood and adolescence which comprise individual characteristics include the age and sex of the young person, their academic ability, measures of affect (Moods and Feelings questionnaire) and psychopathology (DAWBA). Family variables include family type and structure, family adversity, parental involvement in anti-social or delinquent behaviours / dealings with the police and environmental or contextual variables include feelings of security in the neighbourhood (14 years) and also questions about peer relationships asked at various times throughout late childhood / adolescence.

Analysis

The opportunity to use data from the ALSPAC cohort to examine relationships between media exposure (movie and TV viewing ) and risky / delinquent behaviour in adolescence means that both descriptive and inferential analyses can be undertaken to both provide an overview of relationships between these factors and will also provide an opportunity to begin to examine causal relationships.

References

[1] Bandura A, Ross D, Ross SA. Imitation of film mediated aggressive models. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 1963;63:3-11.

[2] Strasburger VC. Children, adolescents and the media: what we know, what we don't know and what we need to find out (quickly!). Archives of Disease in Childhood. 2009;94(9):655-7.

[3] Dal Cin S, Worth K, Gerrard M, Gibbons F, Stoolmiller M, Wills T, et al. Watching and drinking: expectancies, prototypes, and friends' alcohol use mediate the effect of exposure to alcohol use in movies on adolescent drinking. Health Psychology. 2009;28(4):475-83.

[4] Tanski S, Stoolmiller M, Dal Cin S, Worth K, Gibson J, Sargent J. Movie character smoking and adolescent smoking: who matters more, good guys or bad guys? Pediatrics. 2007;124:135-43.

[5] Heatherton T, Sargent J. Does watching smoking in movies promote teenage smoking? Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2009;18(2):63-7.

[6] Boxer P, Huesmann LR, Bushman BJ, O'Brien M, Moceri D. The Role of Violent Media Preference in Cumulative Developmental Risk for Violence and General Aggression. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2009;38(3):417-28.

[7] Anderson CA, Carnagey NL. Causal effects of violent sports video games on aggression: Is it competitiveness or violent content? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2009;45(4):731-9.

[8] Collins RL, Elliott MN, Berry SH, Kanouse DE, Hunter SB. Entertainment television as a healthy sex educator: The impact of condom-efficacy information in an episode of Friends. Pediatrics. 2003;112(5):1115-21.

[9] Chandra A, Martino SC, Collins RL, Elliott MN, Berry SH, Kanouse DE, et al. Does Watching Sex on Television Predict Teen Pregnancy? Findings From a National Longitudinal Survey of Youth. Pediatrics. 2008;122(5):1047-54.

Date proposal received: 
Monday, 26 October, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Monday, 26 October, 2009
Keywords: 
Social Science
Primary keyword: 

B898 - Replication study A common variant on chromosome 11q13 is associated with atopic dermatitis - 26/10/2009

B number: 
B898
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Raquel Granell (University of Bristol, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Prof John Henderson (University of Bristol, UK), Prof George Davey Smith (University of Bristol, UK)
Title of project: 
Replication study: A common variant on chromosome 11q13 is associated with atopic dermatitis.
Proposal summary: 

Common variant (rs7927894) has a reasonably strong effect on atopic dermatitis [1]. We propose two

analyses: 1) to look at the association of this genetic variant with atopic dermatitis in ALSPAC and 2) to look at the association of this genetic variant with asthma, given the effect of filaggrin on asthma.

References

[1] Gordillo et al. A common variant on chromosome 11q13 is associated with atopic dermatitis Nat Genet. 2009 May;41(5):596-601.

Date proposal received: 
Monday, 26 October, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Monday, 26 October, 2009
Keywords: 
Genetics, Skin
Primary keyword: 

B894 - Human Development Epigenetics and Ageing - 23/10/2009

B number: 
B894
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Richard Tremblay (University College Dublin, Europe)
Co-applicants: 
Title of project: 
Human Development, Epigenetics and Ageing.
Proposal summary: 

-Mapping the epigenetic signature of nutritional state and socioeconomic positioning early in life

-Mapping the trajectories of epigenetic signatures at different time points in life and defining their association with nutrition and socioeconomic positioning at the different time points

-Mapping association between epigenetic signatures early in life and throughout life and longevity, define the functional genomic circuitries affected by epigenetic programming early in life that affect longevity.

Our understanding of the process of aging has advanced dramatically in the last decade as a result of elegant genetic work in model organisms, yeast, nematodes and mice. Although the process of aging has its unique characteristics in different branches of the phylogenic tree, common rules have emerged that could now be translated and tested in humans. One of the first principles is the inverse relationship between calorie intake and longevity in organism from different branches of the phylogenic tree. The molecular pathways mediating this involve the insulin response pathway. The second principle is the discovery of the involvement of a class of NAD dependent histone deacetylases the Sirtuins in longevity. Although these HDACs have non-histone targets as well as histone targets the involvement of an HDAC enzyme, alludes to an epigenetic pathway. Indeed elegant genetic analysis by in yeast has linked Sir2 and chromatin modification at telomeres, Sir 2 opposes replicative aging in yeast by maintaining low H4K16 acetylation at telemore elements. Interestingly the activity of Sir2 is dependent and sensitive metabolite in the cell NAD+, thus connecting this epigenetic process with cellular metabolism. The third principle is that the process of aging involves a systemic deregulation of gene expression suggesting a system wide genome wide rearrangement of control of gene expression. Aging seems to represent itself as a component of the normal trajectory of development from conception to death. Longevity is not just determined by the normal process of aging but it is mainly delineated by susceptibility to common adult onset disease such as cardiovascular, metabolic, cancer and mental and cognitive decline. These diseases involve as well system-wide changes in gene expression that affect critical gene-expression circuitries. Animal work from our lab as well as other laboratories suggests that there is an early developmental origin to late onset disease. Thus, adverse exposures early in life will be memorized and appear as adult onset disease that will affect longevity and shorten the life span. Epidemiological data in humans strongly suggests that poverty early in life as well as metabolic disruptions such as nutritional restriction during gestation as well as poverty during the perinatal period increase vulnerability to adult onset diseases including obesity, cardiovascular disease and autoimmune disease.

The critical question is mechanism: How are these early life experiences memorized in the genome so that they trigger disease during adulthood? Indeed can some be triggered by life experiences in the previous generation(s)?

Date proposal received: 
Friday, 23 October, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Friday, 23 October, 2009
Keywords: 
Ageing, Epigenetics , Development
Primary keyword: 

B890 - The associations between early sleep patterns and later risks of Autism and ADHD - 19/10/2009

B number: 
B890
Principal applicant name: 
Prof Alan Emond (University of Bristol, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Dr Paul Gringras (Guy's & St Thomas' Hospital, London, UK), Dr Nicola Scott (Guy's & St Thomas' Hospital, London, UK), Dr Peter Blair (University of Bristol, UK), Dr Joanna Humphries (University of Bristol, UK), Prof John Henderson (University of Bristol, UK), Prof Peter Fleming (University of Bristol, UK)
Title of project: 
The associations between early sleep patterns and later risks of Autism and ADHD.
Proposal summary: 

The children within ALSPAC with diagnoses of ASD and ADHD have been identified from 3 independent sources: (a) the clinical records of all children in the cohort investigated for a suspected developmental disorder by a multi-disciplinary assessment (b) the national educational database in England (PLASC) which identified all children in state schools (over 90% of children) who were needing special educational provision in 2003 and c) the Development and Well Being assessment (DAWBA), applied in the ALSPAC 9 yr research clinic. A total of 86 children were identified with ASD and 175 with ADHD by 11 years of age.

ALSPAC has repeatedly asked questions about children's sleep patterns from birth through to adolescence. A multi disciplinary ALSPAC sleep interest group (Alan Emond, Peter Fleming, Pete Blair, Esther Crawley, Raghu Lingam, Paul Gringras, Nicola Scott and John Henderson) have been overseeing the data. Now that the 16 year old data is available, the dataset is complete through childhood and can be used to answer four crucial sleep-related health questions:

1. What are 'normal' sleep patterns of children in the UK?

We will describe sleep patterns from birth to 16, using latent class analyses and trajectory modelling, and then explore the impact of factors such as co-sleeping, social adversity, parental smoking and alcohol use. This will cover areas including:

Are poor sleepers in infancy, still poor sleepers in adolescence? What impact does social adversity have on sleep patterns?

2. What is the association between short-sleepers and ADHD?

The association between decreased sleep time in infancy and later diagnoses of ADHD is unclear.(1) ALSPAC data allows us to define all the short sleepers in infancy including those that drop their daytime nap early, and then follow-up those children that eventually receive a diagnosis of ADHD. This 'chicken and egg' question could help to provide an early marker of ADHD, help understand causes of ADHD and offer a potential target for early treatment.

3. Is there an association between long-term iron stores, sleep disturbance and ADHD?

Low iron stores, as measured by peripheral ferritin levels have been associated with both restless sleep, and symptoms of ADHD.( 2) The extent of this relationship is still unknown. If it is real, then one would expect increased numbers of children with ADHD to have lower levels of ferritin. Sleep, ferritin levels and ADHD have already been measured within ALSPAC offering a chance to explore another potentially treatable cause of ADHD.

4. What is the relationship between sleep disturbance, breast-feeding and autistic spectrum disorders?

Exciting new genetic research has suggested physiological reasons why sleep problems are so much more common in children with autism than in the general population. (3) Dr Gringras has proposed that due to certain hormones related to early brain development, theoretically, it is non-breast fed infants that should be at increased risk for sleep disorders and autism. ALSPAC, through its questions on early feeding, sleep and later diagnoses of autism has already gathered the data necessary to explore this fascinating question with potentially huge public health implications.

Variables required

In addition to the sleep variables to which we already have access , we are requesting questionnaire data:

1.developmental diagnoses from the IDI database-11 yrs

2. DAWBA data- 7,10 yrs

3.SDQ data-4,6,8, 9, 11yrs

4. SCDC data- 4.5, 6.5, 7.5 yrs

5. IQ data- 8 yrs

6. lab data: ferritins from CIF- 8months,18 months

Project team

The analyses will be supervised by Prof Alan Emond and Dr Paul Gringras, supported by the ALSPAC sleep interest group.

The ALSPAC database is huge, and we a will need a considerable amount of statistical support as this will be the first time all ALSPAC sleep data will have been divided into latent classes, to track each individual child's sleep history and find common patterns. Dr Pete Blair, a senior research statistician at Bristol University, has already worked with aspects of the ALSPAC data and been part of many key publications. His familiarity with the data is a huge advantage, as is his interest in childhood sleep. Pete will be undertaking the analyses, assisted by academic F2 trainee Dr Joanna Humphries.

Whilst Prof Emond, Dr Gringras and the rest of the sleep interest group already have enough ring-fenced academic time to carry out the above work without funding, none of them have Dr Blair's statistical expertise or familiarity with the ALSPAC dataset. We are requesting funding from The Guys Hospital Charitble Trust for Dr Blair's salary for 6 months and a small 'data buddy' fee to gain access to all the cleaned ALSPAC variables required. The only non-pay resources requested are the travel costs associated with meetings of the researchers in London and Bristol. No overheads are included.

The interpretation and writing up of the results will be undertaken by the ALSPAC sleep interest group within existing academic sessions.

REFERENCES

1.Gringras P. Sleep disorders and ADHD. In Taylor, E editor. ADHD. Mackeith Press 2007

2.Konofal E, Lecendreux M, Deron J, Marchand M, Cortese S,Zaim M, Mouren MC, Arnulf I. Effects of iron supplementation on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children.

Pediatr Neurol 2008;38:20-26.

3.J Melke, H Goubran Botros, P Chaste, C Betancur, G Nygren, H Anckarsa , M Rastam,O Stahlberg, IC Gillberg, R Delorme, N Chabane, M-C Mouren m,Simeoni, F Fauchereau, CM Durand, F Chevalier, Drouot, C Collet,J-M Launay M Leboyer C Gillberg T Bourgeron and the PARIS study. Abnormal melatonin synthesis in autism spectrum Disorders. Molecular Psychiatry (2008) 13, 90-98

Date proposal received: 
Monday, 19 October, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Monday, 19 October, 2009
Keywords: 
Autism, Sleep Patterns
Primary keyword: 

B891 - Fathers social interventions and childrens well-being - 16/10/2009

B number: 
B891
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Jonathan Scourfield (University of Cardiff, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Prof David Gunnell (University of Bristol, UK), Prof Geraldine MacDonald (Queens University, Belfast, UK)
Title of project: 
Fathers, social interventions and children's well-being.
Proposal summary: 

The proposed fellowship's main substantive aim is to explore the potential for social interventions with fathers to enhance the emotional well-being of children. Social interventions with fathers are developing fairly rapidly in the UK and elsewhere but the evidence base is slender. (Note that the term 'fathers' is being used here in an inclusive sense, to include social fathers and step fathers as well as biological ones). The proposed fellowship's research programme has three main research questions:

1. How do the social, attitudinal and behavioural characteristics of fathers interact with the emotional well-being of children over time? (and how can this knowledge inform interventions?)

2. What kinds of social interventions are currently being used by practitioners who see themselves as successfully engaging fathers?

3. Can social interventions with fathers improve the emotional well-being of children?

There are three proposed studies which relate to these three questions. Studies 2 and 3 focus directly on social interventions with fathers, relate to research questions 2 and 3 and do not involve ALSPAC data. Study 1 involves analysis of ALSPAC. It relates to research question 1 above and will help to set the context for research on social interventions.

Study 1 uses ALSPAC data to explore associations between children's emotional well-being and the social, behavioural and attitudinal characteristics of fathers. The study will include consideration of which characteristics of fathers are associated with increased or decreased risk of emotional distress in children and any evidence about fathers' contact with helping professionals. It will include examination of whether associations differ in sons and daughters and are independent of maternal influences. There has already been some analysis of ALSPAC data on depression in fathers and absent fathers (e.g Dunn et al, 2004; Ramchandani et al 2008). The current proposal is to examine associations between children's emotional well-being and fathers' attitudes, domestic behaviour and social support (including from professionals), since these are especially relevant to social interventions. Data analysis will involve appropriate multivariable linear and logistic regression modelling to investigate the impact of confounding on observed associations; missing data techniques will be used to assess the impact of missing data as appropriate. The intention is not to replicate the now considerable evidence about father involvement, but to inform social interventions with fathers by identifying the social circumstances and attitudinal and behavioural profiles of fathers which are associated with greater risk of emotional distress in children as well as the characteristics of fathers which seem to be protective factors over time for the maintenance of children's emotional well-being. Much of the existing evidence for a positive impact of father involvement only considers fathers as an undifferentiated category.

The proposed fellowship has been adopted as a DECIPHer study. DECIPHer is the Centre for the Development and Evaluation of Complex Interventions for Public Health Improvement, which involves Cardiff, Bristol and Swansea universities and is funded by the UK Clinical Research Collaboration.

The fellowship application can include the costs of ALSPAC data preparation.

References

Dunn J, Cheng H, O'Connor TG, Bridges L. (2004) Children's perspectives on their relationships with their nonresident fathers: influences, outcomes and implications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45:553-566.

Ramchandani PG, Stein A, O`Connor TG, Heron J, Murray L, Evans J, (2008) Depression in men in the postnatal period and later child psychopathology: a population cohort study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47 (4): 390-8.

Date proposal received: 
Friday, 16 October, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Friday, 16 October, 2009
Keywords: 
Social Science
Primary keyword: 

B888 - Re-introduction of pet ownership questions to the ALSPAC questionnaire PART OF B719 PROJECT - 12/10/2009

B number: 
B888
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Carri Westgarth (University of Liverpool, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Dr Sandra McCune (WALTHAM Centre for Pet Nutrition, UK), Dr Susan Dawson (University of Liverpool, UK)
Title of project: 
Re-introduction of pet ownership questions to the ALSPAC questionnaire (PART OF B719 PROJECT).
Proposal summary: 

The beneficial effect of pet ownership on the physical, social and psychological health of people has been well documented (Friedmann, 1995; Headey, 2003; Katcher, 1981; Katcher & Friedmann, 1982; McNicholas et al., 2005). Contact with companion animals during childhood is likely to have effects on child development , and therefore ownership of pets (and the different types) is also likely to be of significance to child development.

Most studies of health benefits of pets are cross-sectional. Consequently it is difficult to determine whether these health benefits result from pet ownership or whether it is healthier people who choose to own pets. The relatively few longitudinal studies indicate the direction of causality is from pet to owner (Serpell 1991; Headey & Grabka 2007). Longitudinal national representative surveys in Germany and Australia show that pet owners make about 15% fewer annual doctor visits than non-owners (Headey & Grabka 2007). The relationship remains statistically significant after controlling for gender, age, marital status, income and other variables associated with health.

Pet ownership data was collected from the ALSPAC cohort up to age 10 years. This was using the carer questionnaire. At gestation, and 8 months, the number of cats, dogs, rabbits, rodents, birds and other pets were collected. This was then expanded to include categories fish, tortoises and turtles, and any remaining pet types as 'other pets', for the rest of the data collection. These data were collected at 21, 33, 47, 85, 97 and 122 months.

Our team at Liverpool University are currently using the ALSPAC data to explore if there is any association between pet ownership, and obesity, using measures of BMI, fatness and physical activity. This project is funded by WALTHAM(registered trademark) . The data is currently limited to up to 10 years, but it is plausible to consider that there may also be interesting effects beyond this time. In fact, the teenage years may be particularily important in relation to pet ownership, as this is a time when children become more independent and responsible, and for example may walk their dog on their own. For this reason we propose that questions on pet ownership be re-introduced to ALSPAC data collection. The data will be of use not only to researchers interested in obesity and physical activity, but those interested in the effects of the human-companion animal bond in relation to many different aspects of physical and psychological health and development.

To our knowledge, there have been no detailed longitudinal studies decribing pet ownership trends during childhood and teenage years, despite the knowledge that pets, in particular dogs, are more likely to be found in households with school age children that those without (Westgarth et.al., 2007). It is thought that pet ownership is associated with factors such as household type and socioeconomic status, and so there is high potential for confounding in studies of health-related factors. In addition, the factors associated seem to vary with specific pet type, and so the effects of, for example, cat, dog, and small animal ownership need to be investigated separately (Westgarth et.al., In Press). For these reasons it is well worth fully characterising aspects of pet ownership in childhood years using a large and detailed resource such as ALSPAC.

Our proposal to re-introduce pet ownership questions back into the ALSPAC questionnaire at 18 years can be done in a number of formats.

1) Firstly, we would like to re-introduce the basic questions relating to pet ownership, namely - Do you own a pet? And how many cats, dogs, rabbits, rodents, birds, fish, tortoises and terrapins, and other pets, do you own?

2) Because there is a data gap from age 10 to 18, we also propose attempting to inform this gap by asking children to recall whether they owned pets at age 14, and which types. We do not imagine recall bias to be a significant problem, because we imagine that people can reliably remember whether or not they owned a type of pet. They may not be able to recall 'numbers of' a particular pet as accurately though, and therefore a simple indication of pet type is probably most appropriate.

3) Because we have reliable pet ownership information up to 10 years, it would be possible to test the validity of asking respondents to recall pet ownership, by asking them what types of pet they owned up to 10 years old, and then checking this against the data already collected. Surveys of pet ownership are often cross-sectional and ask respondents to recall previous pet ownership, and so a measure of the validity of such an approach would be extremely useful in the field of Human-Animal Interaction research. It would also inform the results from 2) above.

4) The data on pet ownership in the ALSPAC resourse is limited, albeit of large numbers. This would be an opportunity to further explore particular aspects and attitudes relating to pet ownership, that may further elucidate any impact that pet ownership has on health.

In particular:

How much the child is involved in the day-to-day responsibilities and care of the pet.

For example, if they own a dog, do they walk it and how often. This can be compared with the reported frequency of activities such as walking from all children, in particular those not owning a dog.

If they own a dog, what breed, and size of dog.

Measures of attachment to the pet (measured using a published and validated pet attachment scale), as this may affect the impact of owning a pet on a particular individual.

WALTHAM(registered trademark) is a leading scientific authority in pet nutrition and well-being. For almost 50 years they have researched the nutritional and behavioural needs of companion animals and their benefits to humans. In 2008, WALTHAM(registered trademark) formed a public-private partnership with the National Institutes of Health (NIH) in the USA to encourage research in the field of HAI and to help grow the field. In particular they acknowledged the uses of birth cohort studies such as ALSPAC. Sandra McCune directs the Human-Animal Interaction research programme at the WALTHAM(registered trademark) Centre for Pet Nutrition where she manages a large portfolio of HAI research projects. WALTHAM(registered trademark) has identified the role of pets in physical activity and obesity as a research priority for the programme and are keen to support this study.

References

Friedmann, E. (1995). The role of pets in enhancing human well-being: physiological effects. In The Waltham Book of Human-Animal Interaction: Benefits and Responsibilities of Pet Ownership, pp. 33-53. Edited by I. Robinson. Oxford: Elsevier Science Ltd.

Headey, B. (2003). Pet ownership: good for health? Medical Journal Of Australia 179, 460-461.

Headey, B & Grabka, M.W. (2007) Pets and human health in Germany and Australia: National Longitudinal results. Social Indicators Research (2007) 80: 297-311.

Katcher, A. H. (1981). Interactions between people and their pets: form and function. In Interrelations between people and pets, pp. 41-67. Edited by B. Fogle. Springfield: Charles C. Thomas.

Katcher, A. H. & Friedmann, E. (1982). Potential health value of pet ownership. California Veterinarian 36, 9-13.

McNicholas, J., Gilbey, A., Rennie, A., Ahmedzai, S., Dono, J.-A. & Ormerod, E. (2005). Pet ownership and human health: a brief review of evidence and issues. BMJ 331, 1252-1254.

Serpell,J. (1991) Beneficial effects of pet ownership on some aspects of human health and behaviour Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine Volume 84 December: 717-720

Westgarth, C., Pinchbeck, G. L., Bradshaw, J. W., Dawson, S., Gaskell, R. M. & Christley, R. M. (2007). Factors associated with dog ownership and contact with dogs in a UK community. BMC Vet Res 3, 5.

Westgarth, C., Pinchbeck, G. L., Bradshaw, J. W., Dawson, S., Gaskell, R. M. & Christley, R. M. (In Press). Factors associated with cat ownership in a UK community. Vet Record.

Date proposal received: 
Monday, 12 October, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Monday, 12 October, 2009
Keywords: 
Pets
Primary keyword: 

B889 - DCSF funding for increasing participation - 08/10/2009

B number: 
B889
Principal applicant name: 
Deborah Wilson (Department of Children, Families and Schools, London, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Mr Stephen Witt (Department of Children, Families and Schools, London, UK)
Title of project: 
DCSF funding for increasing participation.
Proposal summary: 

The DCSF may have funding available for ALSPAC over the next two financial years to help with increasing participation in the study. There would be £30,000 available in the first year and £55,000 available in the second year, or vice-versa depending on ALSPAC's needs. In order to access the funding, ALSPAC needs to write a proposal to the DCSF outlining how this money will be spent on increasing participation.

As a result of the UWE Social Marketing report and focus groups that were held, a number of ideas of how to increase participation in the study were suggested. Using this advice, ALSPAC want to implement a strategy with the aim of significantly improving participation over the next two years. The first year will focus on using specific marketing techniques to recruit key peers in order to spread the message about ALSPAC. The key peers will be the 'popular' young people who have lots of influence over a wide range of other young people. One idea is for them to comment on ALSPAC on their Facebook page as well as upload photos of ALSPAC events to get their friends interested in what ALSPAC can offer them. ALSPAC will be taking advice from a 'new media' specialist to make sure that the correct message is reaching all young people. ALSPAC will be targeting them as three separate groups; the engaged, recently dropped off and the disengaged. The social marketing report made it very clear that involving these three groups in the study would take three different strategies. For example, the engaged would like to know more about the 'Science', whereas the disengaged at this stage are more interested in incentives. The key idea here is viral marketing and getting it to work for ALSPAC.

The focus in the second year will be on introducing 'club membership' to ALL of the young people in the study for when they turn 18 years old. This is something that all of the young people taking part in the focus groups were very enthusiastic about. The idea behind this is to keep all of the YPs engaged in the study and to give ALSPAC more of a presence in their everyday lives. It would be similar to an NUS card but will be free to join. The young people will be given a membership card which will entitle them to discounts on nationwide shops/cinemas, a free bus/rail card etc and entry to ALSPAC events (e.g. CV writing, fitness assessment, parties and comedy nights) and competitions. More importantly, it will sustain a link between ALSPAC and them. We will be able to keep them updated with what we are doing and what clinics/questionnaires are currently running. For instance, using their membership card, they will be able to use their 'username' to access online questionnaires etc. These are only initial ideas and we expect that as we go along other opportunities will arise that we could take advantage of.

Date proposal received: 
Thursday, 8 October, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Thursday, 8 October, 2009
Keywords: 
Primary keyword: 

B887 - Dietary salt intake in young children - 05/10/2009

B number: 
B887
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Pauline Emmett (University of Bristol, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Dr Janet Warren (Virginia Commonwealth University, USA)
Title of project: 
Dietary salt intake in young children.
Proposal summary: 

Dietary information has been collected repeatedly from a 10% sub-sample of the ALSPAC cohort (around 1000 children) at the following ages: 4 months, 8 months, 1 1/2 years, 3 1/2 years, 5 years and 7 years. Information was collected by a one-day unweighed food record at 4 months, and by a three-day unweighed food record at all other ages. All the dietary data have been coded and prepared and are ready for use. Information on the use of salt at the table and in cooking is available at the following ages: 3 1/2 years, 5 years, and 7 years.

Information is available on a range of socio-demographic and lifestyle factors, including the following: parental education, social class and age, housing tenure, financial difficulties and some questionnaire information about physical activities (e.g. time spent watching TV, walking, cycling etc.).

Table 1 shows mean non-discretionary sodium intakes in ALSPAC from 4 months to 3 1/2 years, in comparison with target average sodium intakes suggested by the Scientific Advisory Committee on Nutrition to the Food Standards Agency.

Table 1. Non-discretionary intakes of sodium (mg) from 4 months to 3 1/2 years and the proportion of children consuming at or below target intakes

____________Percentiles____________ % below

Age Mean (sd) 2.5 25 50 75 97.5 Target target

4 months 180 (59) 92 140 171 210 324 less than 400 99.4

8 months 612 (324) 216 374 546 772 1402 400 28.4

18 months 1432 (439) 706 1140 1384 1668 2472 800 4.9

43 months 1823 (470) 1006 1516 1789 2091 2883 800 0.6

It can be seen that from the age of 8 months the great majority of children are consuming in excess of SACN target intakes for sodium from non-discretionary sodium intake. These figures do not include an estimation of salt added at the table or added to vegetables during cooking. However, the proportion of sodium derived from non-discretionary sources is likely to be lower than it is in adults (15-20%), as most parents do not usually allow their children to add salt at the table, and many report avoiding adding salt when cooking for children. (At age 5 only 15.6% regularly added salt to their children's food in cooking, 47.9% reported never adding it, while only 1.4% allowed their child to add salt at table regularly and 66.1% never did this).

Data from the diaries collected at 4 months suggests that breastfed infants had a lower sodium intake than formula fed infants at this age. Other work has shown a difference in blood pressure at age 7 years between breastfed and formula fed infants.

We will use the data collected in ALSPAC to answer the following research questions:

1. What are the main dietary sources of salt at each age (4m, 8m, 1 1/2y, 2y, 3 1/2y, 5y, 7y)?

2. What food groups distinguish most between children with high and low sodium intakes at each age?

3. How does salt intake relate to intakes of other foods and nutrients relevant to cardiovascular health, and to sociodemographic and lifestyle factors?

4. How well does salt intake "track" throughout early childhood?

5. Do breastfed infants subsequently go on to have lower salt intakes than formula-fed infants.

Date proposal received: 
Monday, 5 October, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Monday, 5 October, 2009
Keywords: 
Diet
Primary keyword: 

B886 - RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN MATERNAL BACKGROUND UVB EXPOSURE AND CORD BLOOD DNA METHYLATION - 05/10/2009

B number: 
B886
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Dave Evans (University of Bristol, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Dr Jon Tobias (University of Bristol, UK), Mr Adrian Sayers (University of Bristol, UK), Dr Caroline Relton (Newcastle University, UK), Prof Debbie A Lawlor (University of Bristol, UK)
Title of project: 
RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN MATERNAL BACKGROUND UVB EXPOSURE AND CORD BLOOD DNA METHYLATION.
Proposal summary: 

Background

We recently reported a positive association in ALSPAC between background levels of UVB in the third trimester of pregnancy, derived from meteorological office records, and subsequent bone development of the child based on DXA scans at age nine (1). UVB exposure as analysed in this way provides a measure of vitamin D status in utero (2), which is in turn thought to contribute to programming of development of several systems within the off-spring including the skeleton. Although the mechanisms which mediate these effects are unknown, one possibility is that these involve altered methylation status of as yet unidentified target genes, by virtue of effects of vitamin D on the activity of enzymes involved in DNA methylation. Following recent methodological advances which enable methylation status to be evaluated at multiple sites across the genome (3), proof of concept studies intended to examine the role of methylation are now feasible.

Hypothesis

Our overall hypothesis is that exposure to low levels of vitamin D in utero has adverse effects on subsequent skeletal development as a consequence of altered methylation of key regulatory genes. The present proposal aims to address one component of this hypothesis, namely that vitamin D exposure in utero affects DNA methylation at multiple sites.

Objectives/Design

1. Approximately 50 DNA sites showing the greatest variability in methylation status will be identified in collaboration with Caroline Relton (University of Newcastle), based on methylation status recently measured at 1500 sites (covering 800 genes) in cord blood samples from 174 ALSPAC children.

2. The genes represented by these sites will be classified according to what biological pathway or process they are likely to affect.

3. Relationships will be analysed between maternal UVB and extent of methylation for each of the 50 methylation sites in turn.

Techniques/approaches

Since last trimester UVB exposure and DNA methylation are both continuous variables, analyses will initially involve linear regression. Further analyses will be performed in which UVB exposure is treated as a dichotomous variable, eg by examining differences in methylation status between those in the upper and lower quartile of UVB (equivalent to birth in late summer and early spring respectively). Additional analyses may also be possible between DNA methylation and maternal vitamin D status in the last trimester in ALSPAC, based on direct measurement of vitamin D which is currently in progress. Whether relationships between maternal vitamin D status and DNA methylation vary according to biological pathway will be addressed by comparing these associations between different categories of genes. Positive findings will subsequently be replicated by repeating these analyses in a cohort from North Cumbria where equivalent information on cord blood DNA methylation and birthdate is available from approximately 200 off spring.

References

1. Sayers A, Tobias JH 2009 Estimated maternal ultraviolet B exposure levels in pregnancy influence skeletal development of the child. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 94:765-771

2. Sayers A, Tilling K, Boucher BJ, Noonan K, Tobias JH 2009 Predicting ambient ultraviolet from routine meteorological data; its potential use as an instrumental variable for vitamin D status in pregnancy in a longitudinal birth cohort in the UK. Int J Epidemiol

3. Kaminsky ZA, Tang T, Wang SC, Ptak C, Oh GH, Wong AH, Feldcamp LA, Virtanen C, Halfvarson J, Tysk C, McRae AF, Visscher PM, Montgomery GW, Gottesman, II, Martin NG, Petronis A 2009 DNA methylation profiles in monozygotic and dizygotic twins. Nat Genet 41:240-245.

Date proposal received: 
Monday, 5 October, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Monday, 5 October, 2009
Keywords: 
Epigenetics , Genetics
Primary keyword: 

B885 - Near Term Birth and Long Term Cognitive Outcomes - 29/09/2009

B number: 
B885
Principal applicant name: 
Dr David Odd (University of Bristol, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Prof Andrew Whitelaw (University of Bristol, UK)
Title of project: 
Near Term Birth and Long Term Cognitive Outcomes.
Proposal summary: 

Aim of the project

The aim of the project is to investigate the relationship between the infants born "near term" (32 to 36 weeks gestation) and IQ.

Background

The long term effects of prematurity are well recorded in infants born before 32 weeks corrected gestation. Extreme prematurity is associated with increased risks of cerebral palsy, speech[1], educational[1], and executive functioning[2] deficits as well as reduction in more global measures of cognition. The long term outcome of infants born at only moderately premature gestations however is less clear. Previous studies have suggested increased rates of educational concerns in infants born before 37 weeks gestation[3, 4] but little evidence exists on specific measures of cognition, particularly IQ, and other measures of neuro-development. At present these infants little specific educational input, and indeed recent recommendations have suggested that that only infants below 32 weeks of gestational age should be offered formal neuro-developmental follow-up.

Experimental design and methods to be used in investigating this problem

Infants born above 31 weeks gestational age will be identified from the ALSPAC cohort. The exposure of interest will be gestational age, categorized as near term (less than 37 weeks corrected gestation) or term (37 weeks or more). The primary outcome will be IQ (as measured by the WISC-II at 8 years). In addition measures of school performance, manual dexterity, attention, memory and language skills will also be assessed.

Covariates

Measures of neonatal wellbeing will be obtained from the STORK dataset (available for all infants born in St Michaels orSouthmeadHospital). Measures used in the analysis will be:

  • Antenatal factors: Gender, maternal parity and maternal hypertension.
  • Intrapartum factors: Birth weight, length and head circumference (corrected for gestation), mode of birth (i.e. spontaneous cephalic, emergency or elective caesarian section, instrumental or breech) and maternal pyrexia.
  • Social factors: Maternal age, socioeconomic group and educational achievements, car ownership, housing tenure, crowding and ethnicity.

Any infants who are admitted to the neonatal unit will have further data on presence or not of neurological signs recorded from the Neonatal Data. Covariates will be added to the model in the blocks above.

Analysis Strategy

Exposure groups will be defined by completed gestational age (32-36 weeks). A reference groups will be defined of infants born at term (37 weeks or more completed weeks) who did not require admission to a neonatal unit.

Outcomes will be both the difference in mean value in the outcome variable (if normally distributed) and the proportion of infants in the lowest 10%. An IQ of less than 80 will be considered 'low'. Linear and logistic regression models will be used to assess the association between the exposure and the outcomes as appropriate.

A secondary analysis will be performed with the results stratified by the completed week of gestation and including all infants born at term (including those admitted to the neonatal units).

Chained Equations may be used to impute the value of any missing covariate data. All analysis will be performed using STATA 10 software (Stata Corp, TX,USA).

Study power

Assuming that around 5% of infants will be born moderately preterm and that there is IQ data available on 6000 infants within the cohort, we would have a power of greater than 99% to find a clinically important difference of 3.25 IQ points (a quarter of one standard deviation) with an alpha of 5%.

References

1. Wolke D, Samara M, Bracewell M, Marlow N, Group EPS: Specific language difficulties and school achievement in children born at 25 weeks of gestation or less.[see comment]. Journal of Pediatrics 2008;152:256-262.

2. Marlow N, Hennessy EM, Bracewell MA, Wolke D, Group EPS: Motor and executive function at 6 years of age after extremely preterm birth. Pediatrics 2007;120:793-804.

3. Chyi LJ, Lee HC, Hintz SR, Gould JB, Sutcliffe TL: School outcomes of late preterm infants: special needs and challenges for infants born at 32 to 36 weeks gestation. J Pediatr 2008;153:25-31.

Date proposal received: 
Tuesday, 29 September, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Tuesday, 29 September, 2009
Keywords: 
Cognitive Function
Primary keyword: 

B884 - Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies of Eczema - 24/09/2009

B number: 
B884
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Dave Evans (University of Bristol, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Prof John Henderson (University of Bristol, UK), Prof Tim Spector (King's College London, UK), Prof Nick Martin (Queensland Institute of Medical Research, ROW)
Title of project: 
Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies of Eczema.
Proposal summary: 

Eczema is due to multiple genetic and environmental factors. Although variants in the fillagrin gene have been robustly associated with eczema risk, the vast majority of the genetic variance in this condition remains to be explained. It is our aim to construct a variable indicating whether an individual has ever suffered from eczema and to subject this variable to genome-wide association analysis. Similar data is also available from twins UK (Tim Spector) and Queensland Institute of Medical Research (Nick Martin) and both groups have expressed interest in the possibility of pooling results in a meta-analysis. We will aim to follow up significant associations in the remainder of the ALSPAC cohort and other cohorts for which this measure is available.

Date proposal received: 
Thursday, 24 September, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Thursday, 24 September, 2009
Keywords: 
GWAS, Skin
Primary keyword: 

B880 - The association of vitamin D with cognitive function in childhood - 23/09/2009

B number: 
B880
Principal applicant name: 
Prof Debbie A Lawlor (University of Bristol, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Dr Iain Lang (Peninsula Medical School, University of Plymouth, UK), Dr David Llewellyn (Peninsula Medical School, University of Plymouth, UK)
Title of project: 
The association of vitamin D with cognitive function in childhood.
Proposal summary: 

Background: The role of vitamin D in calcium metabolism and bone health is well documented. Vitamin D enables normal bone mineralization and prevents hypocalcemic tetany. It is also necessary for bone growth and bone remodeling by osteoblasts and osteoclasts. Vitamin D deficiency is common and typically arises from inadequate intake, inadequate sunlight exposure, and limited absorption or conversion into active metabolites (often due to liver or kidney disorders). Recent studies suggest that vitamin D influences various health conditions (such as diabetes, heart disease and hypertension) and mortality. Accumulating evidence also suggests previously unsuspected roles for vitamin D in brain development and neuroprotection. Recent population-based studies link vitamin D deficiency in old age with cognitive function cross-sectionally (Buell, et al., 2009; Lee, et al., 2009; Llewellyn, et al., 2009) and prospectively (Llewellyn, et al., in review). Further research is needed to establish whether vitamin D deficiency influences cognitive development in children.

The mechanisms underlying these associations are unclear, and residual confounding is possible, particularly in cross-sectional studies. Vitamin D receptors are present in a wide variety of cells, including neurons and glial cells, and genes encoding the enzymes involved in the metabolism of vitamin D are also expressed in the brain (McCann & Ames, 2008). In a recent review, Buell and Dawson-Hughes (2008) emphasize that Vitamin D may be neuroprotective through antioxidative mechanisms, immunomodulation, neuronal calcium regulation, detoxification mechanisms and enhanced nerve conduction. Vitamin D may play a role in brain detoxification pathways by reducing cellular calcium, inhibiting the synthesis of inducible nitric oxide synthase and increasing levels of the antioxidant glutathione (Garcion, 2002). Vitamin D stimulates neurogenesis and regulates the synthesis of neurotrophic factors which are important for cell differentiation and survival (Brown, et al., 2003). Vitamin D is also an immunosuppressor and may inhibit autoimmune damage to the nervous system (Garcion, 2002). Calcitriol stimulates amyloid-beta phagocytosis and clearance while protecting against apoptosis (Masoumi, et al., 2009). Low levels of vitamin D may also be associated with an increased risk of neurological diseases such as multiple sclerosis (Munger, et al., 2006), and Parkinson's (Newmark & Newmark, 2007).

The majority of brain development occurs in utero and during childhood. Optimal cognitive development in early-life may confer resistance to late-life cognitive decline and dementia in late life through 'cognitive reserve' and higher levels of midlife functioning. To our knowledge no prospective study has examined the association between in utero or early childhood vitamin D levels and childhood cognition or educational attainment. We also intend to examine whether genetic variants associated with vitamin D levels are also associated with cognitive function in childhood using an instrumental variables approach.

We would envisage the following series of papers to result from this work:

1) Association of childhood vitamin D with childhood intelligence.

2) Association of childhood vitamin D with educational attainment (key stage and GCSE) and the possible mediation by intelligence.

3) Association of maternal vitamin D during pregnancy with childhood intelligence and educational attainment (taking into account childhood vitamin D levels).

4) Possible neuroimaging studies (in collaboration with Tomas Paus and others) to further explore the above.

This work fits very well with Debbie Lawlor's MRC funded vitamin D grant which had as a main focus vascular and metabolic outcomes but noted that we would also examine relevant associations with a wide range of other health outcomes for which causal claims for vitamin D have been made. In this respect bone and respiratory/atopy phenotypes were specifically mentioned (in the grant and original proposal to exec.) but IQ and educational outcomes were not and therefore we are submitting this request here.

Methods

Existing data and planned (funded) vitamin D assays will be used. Analyses will be completed by Anna Tolpeinan with supervison by Debbie Lawlor and input (including development of analysis plan) from other applicants. Our preliminary analysis plan is to use multivariable regression models with adjustment for potential confounders. Potential mediators will be added as additional terms to fully adjusted models. Debbie Lawlor has access to all necessary data except educational attainment. We are aware that these data have been funded by the ESRC large grant and would be very keen to collaborate with the key person involved in the funding and collection of those data.

Date proposal received: 
Wednesday, 23 September, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Wednesday, 23 September, 2009
Keywords: 
Cognitive Function, Vitamin D
Primary keyword: 

B879 - The association of childhood sedentary behaviour with cardiovascular risk factors - 23/09/2009

B number: 
B879
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Emmanuel Stamatakis (University College London, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Prof Debbie A Lawlor (University of Bristol, UK), Prof Chris Riddoch (University of Bath, UK), Prof Naveed Sattar (University of Glasgow, UK)
Title of project: 
The association of childhood sedentary behaviour with cardiovascular risk factors.
Proposal summary: 

Among adults, excessive sedentary behaviour (as characterised by those activities that involve sitting, in particular screen (TV or computer) viewing) is linked to increased risk for cardiovascular events,(1,2) all-cause mortality,(1) obesity,(3,4) dyslipidemia,(5) higher plasma glucose levels,(6) and the metabolic syndrome.(7) These links seem to be independent of participation in moderate-to-vigorous physical activity (MVPA). For example, among adults who meet the MVPA guidelines, significant, detrimental dose-response associations of sedentary time exist with all-cause and cardiovascular mortality,(1) waist circumference and BMI,(3) systolic blood pressure, 2-hour plasma glucose, fasting plasma glucose, triglycerides, and HDL cholesterol.(8)

In children, it is less clear how sedentary behaviour relates to cardiovascular and metabolic health. There is cross-sectional evidence that sedentary behaviours, such as TV viewing relates to cardiometabolic risk,(9,10) but these cross-sectional relationships are not always independent of MVPA, especially when sedentary behaviour and MVP are measured objectively,(11) raising the possibility that research based on non-objective measurements is biased. However, cross-sectional evidence may be explained by reverse causation, for example there are indications that body weight status can predict sedentary behaviour but sedentary behaviour may not predict future obesity in adults.(12) There is very little work done on the longitudinal relationships of objectively measured sedentary behaviour and cardio-metabolic health indicators in children. Such evidence will provide clues as to whether sedentary behaviour should be targeted separately to MVPA to protect and improve children's health. If sedentary time is found to be independently associated with cardio-metabolic risk in children, UK physical activity recommendations should explicitly address sitting in addition to recommending 60 minutes of MVPA a day. Such recommendations currently exist in guidelines in USA and Canada(13) and Australia,(14) where it is recommended that children limit their TV viewing time to less than 2 hours per day.

The main purpose of the proposed project will therefore be to study the longitudinal associations between sedentary behaviour and cardio-metabolic risk factors in children with an emphasis on the interactions between MVPA. Specific objectives would be:

* To examine the association of sedentary behaviour (assessed by questionnaire and accelerometer) in earlier childhood with variation in fasting blood glucose, insulin & lipids assessed at age 15 and 17 years.

. To examine the extent to which any of these associations are mediated by adiposity

. To examine whether associations of sedentary behaviour with these metabolic risk factors varies by whether the person has high or low levels of MVPA

Manos Stamatakis is applying for a Wellcome Trust senior career scientist fellowship to address these issues using data from a number of cohort studies and National cross-sectional surveys. These are likely to include (permissions either obtained or currently being sought) the Health Survey for England, Whitehall II and British Women's Heart and Health Study. One overall aim is to examine how patterns of sedentary behaviour and their associations vary by age group. Relevant analyses in ALSPAC as one part of his planned work for this fellowship. This will use data already available/funded in ALSPAC, including collected accelerometer data (Chris Riddoch grants) and fasting blood sample data (Debbie Lawlor grants) and will not require new data collection or bio-assays.

This application has been discussed with Prof C Riddoch & we would work in close collaboration with him & with Professors S Blair & R Pate who are applicants on the grant that funds the accelerometer data.

Methods

All applicants will develop the analysis protocol, in collaboration with S Blair & R Pate

DAL will put together the dataset

MS will complete analyses

All applicants will contribute to papers for publication

References

1. Katzmarzyk PT, Church TS, Craig CL, Bouchard C. Sitting Time and Mortality from All Causes, Cardiovascular Disease, and Cancer. Med Sci Sports Exe. 2009;41:998-1005.

2. Stamatakis E, Hamer M, Dunstan DW. Television and other screen-based entertainment time and cardiovascular events: population-based study with ongoing mortality and hospital events follow up. Under Review

3. Stamatakis E, Hirani V, Rennie K. Moderate-to-vigorous physical activity and sedentary behaviours in relation to body mass index-defined and waist circumference-defined obesity. Br J Nutrit 2009;101:765-773.

4. Frank LD, Martin A. Andresen MA, et al. Obesity Relationships with Community Design, Physical Activity, and Time Spent in Cars. Am J Prev Med 2004;27:87-96.

5. Jakes RW, Day NE, Khaw KT, et al. Television viewing and low participation in vigorous recreation are independently associated with obesity and markers of cardiovascular disease risk: EPIC-Norfolk population-based study. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2003;57:1089-1096.

6. Healy GN, Dunstan DW, Salmon J, et al. Objectively Measured Light-Intensity Physical Activity Is Independently Associated With 2-h Plasma Glucose. Diabetes Care. 2007;30:1384-1389.

7. Chang PC, Li TC, Wu MT, et al. Association between television viewing and the risk of metabolic syndrome in a community based population. BMC Public Health 2008;8:193 doi: 10.1186/1471-2458/8/193.

8. Healy GN, Dunstan DW, Salmon J, et al.: Television time and continuous metabolic risk in physically active adults. Med Sci Sports Exerc 2008, 40:639-645

9. Ekelund, U; Brage, S; Froberg, K; et al. TV viewing and physical activity are independently associated with metabolic risk in children: The European Youth Heart Study. PLoS Medicine 2006; 3: 2449-2457.

10. Martinez-Gomez, D; Tucker, J; Heelan, KA; et al. Associations Between Sedentary Behavior and Blood Pressure in Young Children. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine 2009, 163 (8): 724-730.

11. Mitchell JA, Mattocks C, Ness AR, et al. Sedentary Behavior and Obesity in a Large Cohort of Children. Obesity 2009; 17: 1596-1602

12. Ekelund U, Brage S, Besson H, Sharp S, Wareham NJ. Time spent being sedentary and weight gain in healthy adults: reverse or bidirectional causality? Am J Clin Nutr. 2008;88:612-617.

13. American Academy of Pediatrics, Committee on Public Education. Children, adolescents, and television. Pediatrics. 2001;107:423-426.

14. Australian Government, Department of Health and Ageing. Australia's physical activity recommendations for 5-12 tear olds. Department of Health and Ageing 2004, Canberra, Australia.

Date proposal received: 
Wednesday, 23 September, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Wednesday, 23 September, 2009
Keywords: 
Cardiovascular
Primary keyword: 

B878 - Psychosocial Stress Growth Tempo and Pubertal Timing - 23/09/2009

B number: 
B878
Principal applicant name: 
Prof Ren?e Danielle Boynton-Jarrett (Boston University, USA)
Co-applicants: 
Dr Michele Marcus (Emory University, USA), Dr Mildred Maisonet (Emory University, USA)
Title of project: 
Psychosocial Stress, Growth Tempo, and Pubertal Timing.
Proposal summary: 

A building body of research evidence suggests that intrauterine and early life exposures contribute to chronic disease risk in later life. While recent evidence supports the role of childhood adversities on reproductive lifespan,1-4 yet there is a limited understanding of mechanisms through which these adversities impact health. Researchers have established an association between psychosocial stressors in childhood and disruption of pubertal timing. The strongest literature is among girls, and using age at menarche (AAM) as a proxy measure for pubertal timing. Earlier age at menarche has been linked to all cause mortality,5 risk for breast cancer,6-8 cardiovascular disease,9 depression,10 metabolic syndrome11 and associated features, including overweight/obesity,12,13 insulin resistance,14 and polycystic ovarian syndrome,10 while later age at menarche is associated with depression,15,16 fractures,17-19 and lower bone mineral density.20-22 To date there has been limited exploration of psychosocial predictors of pubertal onset and the tempo of pubertal development among both girls and boys.

Psychosocial antecedents of age of menarche include: familial conflict,23 alterations in family structure,24 stressful home circumstances,25-27 paternal absence in childhood,28 and quality of familial relationships.29,30 3,25,31-34 A growing number of studies have demonstrated an association between child abuse and age at menarche.3,35-39 In the five studies to date sexual abuse has demonstrated a stronger and more consistent association with early menarche while physical abuse has had a weakly positive association in some studies.3,35-39 A shared limitation of these studies is the failure to account for additional childhood hardships, such as poverty, that are also associated with pubertal development.

The intent of this study is to investigate the independent and cumulative impact of multiple types of childhood adversities (including child abuse, neglect, family conflict, and socioeconomic status) on timing of puberty in this prospective birth cohort. We hypothesized that children exposed to higher cumulative adversities will be more likely to have altered timing of menarche-earlier or later age at onset. We proposed to investigate the relation between exposure to early life adversities and the tempo of pubertal development and timing of puberty. Specifically, we are interested in exploring the impact of exposure to physical abuse, sexual abuse, emotional abuse, neglect, and/or family conflict and the timing and tempo of pubertal development. We hypothesize that childhood adversities may impact pubertal development through influences on rate of change in BMI over time, lifestyles and health behaviours, including dietary habits, physical activity, and sedentary behaviours. Existing studies are limited by retrospective exposure assessment, and failure to consider multiple types of adversities.

Thus, a proposed mechanism linking exposure to child abuse and sexual maturation is through dysregulation of neuroendocrine systems that respond to environmental challenges (such as the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA), hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal (HPG) axes. Child maltreatment is a significant psychosocial stressor and therefore may influence responsiveness of the HPA and HPG axes, neuroendocrine functioning, hormonal milieu and ultimately influence timing of sexual maturation. In addition, child abuse may be associated with nutritional factors, energy expenditure, level of activity, and health behaviors in early life that impact AAM. Compelling evidence from several recent studies suggest a relation between childhood adversities and body mass index (BMI) in adulthood.40,41 Child maltreatment has been linked to both excessive weight control and obesity,40-47 and disordered eating.48-53

A second area of interest is the relation between pubertal development and disordered eating patterns and body dissatisfaction. Puberty is one commonly cited risk factor for disordered eating, body dissatisfaction, and eating disorders.10,54 Past studies which have investigated the timing of puberty in relation to the development of eating disorders, and body dissatisfaction have produced mixed findings.54-58 A common limitation shared by previous studies includes the use of cross-sectional methodology, clinical samples, or small community samples. The advantage of the AVON study is that it is robust and comprehensive in the measurement of disordered eating and puberty, the longitudinal methodology, sample size, and sample population. This secondary analysis will focus on the impact of characteristics of pubertal development, particularly timing and tempo, on patterns of disordered eating, eating disorders, and degree of body dissatisfaction.

Please see the attached Excel spreadsheet which outlines the concepts, measures, source, and time points requested (as outlined in application item #8).

References

1. Mishra GD, Cooper R, Tom SE, Kuh D. Early life circumstances and their impact on menarche and menopause. Womens Health (Lond Engl) 2009;5(2):175-90.

2. Parent AS, Teilmann G, Juul A, Skakkebaek NE, Toppari J, Bourguignon JP. The timing of normal puberty and the age limits of sexual precocity: variations around the world, secular trends, and changes after migration. Endocr Rev 2003;24(5):668-93.

3. Romans SE, Martin JM, Gendall K, Herbison GP. Age of menarche: the role of some psychosocial factors. Psychol Med 2003;33(5):933-9.

4. Slyper AH. The pubertal timing controversy in the USA, and a review of possible causative factors for the advance in timing of onset of puberty. Clin Endocrinol (Oxf) 2006;65(1):1-8.

5. Jacobsen BK, Heuch I, Kvale G. Association of low age at menarche with increased all-cause mortality: a 37-year follow-up of 61,319 Norwegian women. Am J Epidemiol 2007;166(12):1431-7.

6. Hamilton AS, Mack TM. Puberty and genetic susceptibility to breast cancer in a case-control study in twins. N Engl J Med 2003;348(23):2313-22.

7. Bernstein L. Epidemiology of endocrine-related risk factors for breast cancer. J Mammary Gland Biol Neoplasia 2002;7(1):3-15.

8. Romundstad PR, Vatten LJ, Nilsen TI, Holmen TL, Hsieh CC, Trichopoulos D, Stuver SO. Birth size in relation to age at menarche and adolescent body size: implications for breast cancer risk. Int J Cancer 2003;105(3):400-3.

9. Remsberg KE, Demerath EW, Schubert CM, Chumlea WC, Sun SS, Siervogel RM. Early menarche and the development of cardiovascular disease risk factors in adolescent girls: the Fels Longitudinal Study. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2005;90(5):2718-24.

10. Stice E, Presnell K, Bearman SK. Relation of early menarche to depression, eating disorders, substance abuse, and comorbid psychopathology among adolescent girls. Dev Psychol 2001;37(5):608-19.

11. Frontini MG, Srinivasan SR, Berenson GS. Longitudinal changes in risk variables underlying metabolic Syndrome X from childhood to young adulthood in female subjects with a history of early menarche: the Bogalusa Heart Study.Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 2003;27(11):1398-404.

12. Kaplowitz PB, Slora EJ, Wasserman RC, Pedlow SE, Herman-Giddens ME.Earlier onset of puberty in girls: relation to increased body mass index and race. Pediatrics 2001;108(2):347-53.

13. Adair LS, Gordon-Larsen P. Maturational timing and overweight prevalence in US adolescent girls. Am J Public Health 2001;91(4):642-4.

14. Ibanez L, Potau N, de Zegher F. Recognition of a new association: reduced fetal growth, precocious pubarche, hyperinsulinism and ovarian dysfunction. Ann Endocrinol (Paris) 2000;61(2):141-2.

15. Bisaga K, Petkova E, Cheng J, Davies M, Feldman JF, Whitaker AH. Menstrual functioning and psychopathology in a county-wide population of high school girls. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2002;41(10):1197-204.

16. Herva A, Jokelainen J, Pouta A, Veijola J, Timonen M, Karvonen JT, Joukamaa M. Age at menarche and depression at the age of 31 years: findings from the Northern Finland 1966 Birth Cohort Study. J Psychosom Res 2004;57(4):359-62.

17. Fujiwara S, Kasagi F, Yamada M, Kodama K. Risk factors for hip fracture in a Japanese cohort. J Bone Miner Res 1997;12(7):998-1004.

18. Roy DK, O'Neill TW, Finn JD, Lunt M, Silman AJ, Felsenberg D, Armbrecht G, Banzer D, Benevolenskaya LI, Bhalla A, Bruges Armas J, Cannata JB, Cooper C, Dequeker J, Diaz MN, Eastell R, Yershova OB, Felsch B, Gowin W, Havelka S, Hoszowski K, Ismail AA, Jajic I, Janott I, Johnell O, Kanis JA, Kragl G, Lopez Vaz A, Lorenc R, Lyritis G, Masaryk P, Matthis C, Miazgowski T, Gennari C, Pols HA, Poor G, Raspe HH, Reid DM, Reisinger W, Scheidt-Nave C, Stepan JJ, Todd CJ, Weber K, Woolf AD, Reeve J. Determinants of incident vertebral fracture in men and women: results from the European Prospective Osteoporosis Study (EPOS). Osteoporos Int 2003;14(1):19-26.

19. Silman AJ. Risk factors for Colles' fracture in men and women: results from the European Prospective Osteoporosis Study. Osteoporos Int 2003;14(3):213-8.

20. Rosenthal DI, Mayo-Smith W, Hayes CW, Khurana JS, Biller BM, Neer RM, Klibanski A. Age and bone mass in premenopausal women. J Bone Miner Res 1989;4(4):533-8.

21. Varenna M, Binelli L, Zucchi F, Ghiringhelli D, Gallazzi M, Sinigaglia L. Prevalence of osteoporosis by educational level in a cohort of postmenopausal women. Osteoporos Int 1999;9(3):236-41.

22. Eastell R. Role of oestrogen in the regulation of bone turnover at the menarche. J Endocrinol 2005;185(2):223-34.

23. Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Belsky J, Silva PA. Childhood experience and the onset of menarche: a test of a sociobiological model. Child Dev 1992;63(1):47-58.

24. Pesonen AK, Raikkonen K, Heinonen K, Kajantie E, Forsen T, Eriksson JG. Reproductive traits following a parent-child separation trauma during childhood: a natural experiment during World War II. Am J Hum Biol 2008;20(3):345-51.

25. Wierson M, Long PJ, Forehand RL. Toward a new understanding of early menarche: the role of environmental stress in pubertal timing. Adolescence 1993;28(112):913-24.

26. Ellis BJ, Essex MJ. Family environments, adrenarche, and sexual maturation: a longitudinal test of a life history model. Child Dev 2007;78(6):1799-817.

27. Bogaert AF. Age at puberty and father absence in a national probability sample. J Adolesc 2005;28(4):541-6.

28. Belsky J, Steinberg L, Draper P. Childhood experience, interpersonal development, and reproductive strategy: and evolutionary theory of socialization. Child Dev 1991;62(4):647-70.

29. Ellis BJ, Garber J. Psychosocial antecedents of variation in girls' pubertal timing: maternal depression, stepfather presence, and marital and family stress. Child Dev 2000;71(2):485-501.

30. Ellis BJ, McFadyen-Ketchum S, Dodge KA, Pettit GS, Bates JE. Quality of early family relationships and individual differences in the timing of pubertal maturation in girls: a longitudinal test of an evolutionary model. J Pers Soc Psychol 1999;77(2):387-401.

31. Maestripieri D, Roney JR, DeBias N, Durante KM, Spaepen GM. Father absence, menarche and interest in infants among adolescent girls. Dev Sci 2004;7(5):560-6.

32. Saxbe DE, Repetti RL. Brief report: Fathers' and mothers' marital relationship predicts daughters' pubertal development two years later. J Adolesc 2008.

33. Tither JM, Ellis BJ. Impact of fathers on daughters' age at menarche: a genetically and environmentally controlled sibling study. Dev Psychol 2008;44(5):1409-20.

34. Mendle J, Turkheimer E, D'Onofrio BM, Lynch SK, Emery RE, Slutske WS, Martin NG. Family structure and age at menarche: a children-of-twins approach. Dev Psychol 2006;42(3):533-42.

35. Brown J, Cohen P, Chen H, Smailes E, Johnson JG. Sexual trajectories of abused and neglected youths. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2004;25(2):77-82.

36. Foster H, Hagan J, Brooks-Gunn J. Growing up fast: stress exposure and subjective "weathering" in emerging adulthood. J Health Soc Behav 2008;49(2):162-77.

37. Vigil JM, Geary DC, Byrd-Craven J. A life history assessment of early childhood sexual abuse in women. Dev Psychol 2005;41(3):553-61.

38. Wise LA, Palmer JR, Rothman EF, Rosenberg L. Childhood Abuse and Early Menarche: Findings From the Black Women's Health Study. Am J Public Health 2009.

39. Zabin LS, Emerson MR, Rowland DL. Childhood sexual abuse and early menarche: the direction of their relationship and its implications. J Adolesc Health 2005;36(5):393-400.

40. Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, Williamson DF, Spitz AM, Edwards V, Koss MP, Marks JS. Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults. The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. Am J Prev Med 1998;14(4):245-58.

41. Williamson DF, Thompson TJ, Anda RF, Dietz WH, Felitti V. Body weight and obesity in adults and self-reported abuse in childhood.Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 2002;26(8):1075-82.

42. Lissau I, Sorensen TI.Parental neglect during childhood and increased risk of obesity in young adulthood. Lancet 1994;343(8893):324-7.

43. Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, Williamson DF, Spitz AM, Edwards V, Koss MP, Marks JS. Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults. The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. Am J Prev Med 1998;14(4):245-58.

44. Gunstad J, Paul RH, Spitznagel MB, Cohen RA, Williams LM, Kohn M, Gordon E. Exposure to early life trauma is associated with adult obesity. Psychiatry Res 2006;142(1):31-7.

45. Lissau I, Sorensen TI. Parental neglect during childhood and increased risk of obesity in young adulthood. Lancet 1994;343(8893):324-7.

46. Noll JG, Zeller MH, Trickett PK, Putnam FW. Obesity risk for female victims of childhood sexual abuse: a prospective study. Pediatrics 2007;120(1):e61-7.

47. Williamson DF, Thompson TJ, Anda RF, Dietz WH, Felitti V. Body weight and obesity in adults and self-reported abuse in childhood. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 2002;26(8):1075-82.

48. Johnson J, Cohen P, Kasen S, Brook JS. Childhood adversities associated with risk for eating disorders or weight problems during adolescence or early adulthood. American Journal of Psychiatry 2002;159(3):394-400.

49. Wonderlich SA, Wilsnack RW, Wilsnack SC, Harris TR. Childhood sexual abuse and bulimic behavior in a nationally representative sample. Am J Public Health 1996;86:1082-1086.

50. Everill JT, Waller G. Reported sexual abuse and eating psychopathology: a review of the evidence for a causal link. Int J Eat Disord. 1995;18:1-11.

51. Welch SL, Fairburn CG. Childhood sexual and physical abuse as risk factors for the development of bulimia nervosa. Child Abuse Negl. 1996;20:633-642.

52. Ackard DM, Neumark-Sztainer D, Hannan PJ, French S, Story M. Binge and purge behavior among adolescents: associations with sexual and physical abuse in a nationally representative sample: the Commonwealth Fund survey. Child Abuse Negl 2001;25(6):771-85.

53. Fonseca H, Ireland M, Resnick MD. Familial correlates of extreme weight control behaviors among adolescents. Int J Eat Disord 2002;32(4):441-8.

54. Patton GC, Viner R. Pubertal transitions in health. Lancet 2007;369(9567):1130-9.

55. Ackard DM, Peterson CB. Association between puberty and disordered eating, body image, and other psychological variables. Int J Eat Disord 2001;29(2):187-94.

56. Kaltiala-Heino R, Rimpela M, Rissanen A, Rantanen P. Early puberty and early sexual activity are associated with bulimic-type eating pathology in middle adolescence. J Adolesc Health 2001;28(4):346-52.

57. Killen JD, Hayward C, Litt I, Hammer LD, Wilson DM, Miner B, Taylor CB, Varady A, Shisslak C. Is puberty a risk factor for eating disorders? Am J Dis Child 1992;146(3):323-5.

58. Mangweth-Matzek B, Rupp CI, Hausmann A, Kemmler G, Biebl W. Menarche, puberty, and first sexual activities in eating-disordered patients as compared with a psychiatric and a nonpsychiatric control group. Int J Eat Disord 2007;40(8):705-10.

Date proposal received: 
Wednesday, 23 September, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Wednesday, 23 September, 2009
Keywords: 
Growth, Puberty, Stress
Primary keyword: 

B882 - Pathways to Alcohol Us Disorders in ALSPAC A Genetic-Developmental Study - 22/09/2009

B number: 
B882
Principal applicant name: 
Prof Kenneth Kendler (Virginia Commonwealth University, USA)
Co-applicants: 
Dr Danielle Dick (Virginia Commonwealth University, USA), Dr Alexis Edwards (Virginia Commonwealth University, USA)
Title of project: 
Pathways to Alcohol Us Disorders in ALSPAC: A Genetic-Developmental Study.
Proposal summary: 

There is considerable evidence suggesting that a wide range of childhood and adolescent externalizing behaviors are prospectively associated with the subsequent development of alcohol use (AU) and alcohol use problems (AUPs), including several dimensions on which data have been collected within the ALSPAC study: e.g., conduct problems, temperament, personality, sensation seeking, and impulsivity. Clearly, we would not expect these various aspects of externalizing behavior to act independently, but rather to operate on AU and AUPs through a dynamic system of influences, both within and across time. For example, early temperament has been shown to influence later conduct problems, for which there are also known associations with aspects of personality, impulsivity, and sensation seeking. There is little clarity, however, regarding how mechanistic associations between these facets ultimately influence AU and AUPs in adolescence and adulthood. Prior to examining the structural nature of these relationships, we must first establish the factor structures within each of the dimensions, independently. So, for example, ALSPAC has collected information on conduct problems assessed across several points in time, and with multiple instruments (e.g., the Revised Rutter Parent Scale for Preschool Children at 42 months and repeated assessments of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire at ages 6.5, 9.5, and 11.5 years). In order to assess development within conduct problems, we must first confirm that the two scales tap into the same underlying construct. Toward this end, one strategy that we propose is to identify a set of "anchor" items/behaviors that are assessed within both measures (e.g., is inconsiderate of others, steals, destroys/damages property, fights, bullies, lies/cheats). We can then subject this common set of variables to within-wave confirmatory factor analysis, allowing us to differentiate between items that yield consistent parameter estimates across time (equivalent factor loadings across assessments) and those for which associations with the underlying construct vary across time (i.e., some items may reflect behavior that is more or less indicative of conduct problems at different stages of development). Time-specific factor score estimates (FSEs) can then be derived by specifying a model which equates factor loadings across time for those items displaying factorial invariance, while freely estimating loadings at each wave of assessment for those items displaying developmental inconsistency. Moreover, we propose to implement these procedures across the different dimensions of externalizing behavior that have been conducted in ALSPAC. The resulting FSEs, which capitalize on consistency while also recognizing the potential for specific behaviors to increase/decrease in relevance across development, can then be used as the unit of measurement in models addressing the complex structural relationships between individual dimensions of behavior, general domains of risk, and alcohol-related outcomes. Accordingly, the broad goal of this set of analyses will be to characterize the measurement of, and inter-relationships between, different domains of externalizing behavior that have been assessed in ALSPAC, in order to use this information to study how different facets of externalizing behavior relate to alcohol use. We have previously had a conference call with Barbara Maughan (and Glyn Lewis) and discussed her interesting work on trajectories of conduct problems and externalizing behavior in the ALSPAC sample, so we would plan to work with her (and other interested ALSPAC collaborators) on these efforts, so as to ensure there is no duplication of effort across the projects. Since our primary interest is in understanding how externalizing behavior relates to alcohol use (with the latter being our primary outcome of interest), and her interest is in the nature of externalizing behavior itself (her primary outcome of interest), we believe that our projects will be synergistic rather than overlapping in scope.

Date proposal received: 
Tuesday, 22 September, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Tuesday, 22 September, 2009
Keywords: 
Alcohol
Primary keyword: 

B876 - Association analysis between a high risk autism SNP and the broader autism phenotype in the ALSPAC cohort - 21/09/2009

B number: 
B876
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Beate St. Pourcain (University of Bristol, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Prof George Davey Smith (University of Bristol, UK)
Title of project: 
Association analysis between a high risk autism SNP and the broader autism phenotype in the ALSPAC cohort.
Proposal summary: 

Recent genome-wide analysis identified a genetic variant on 5p14.1 (rs4307059), which is associated with risk for autism spectrum disorder (ASD). We recently investigated up to 7313 ALSPAC children and studied association between rs4307059 and potential broader autism traits including early vocabulary and communication (MacArthur-Communicative-Development-Inventory/MacArthur-Toddler-Communication-Questionnaire; Denver-Developmental-Screening-Test), social behaviour and behavioural difficulties (Emotionality-Activity-Sociability-Temperament-Survey; Revised-Rutter-Parent-Scale for Preschool Children; Standard-Assessment-Tests of social skills; Strengths-and-Difficulties-Questionnaire, Cambridge Hormones-and-Moods-Project-Friendship-Questionnaire; Special-Educational-Needs assessment), social communication (Social-and-Communications-Disorders-Checklist; Children's-Communication-Checklist) and verbal intelligence (Wechsler-Intelligence-Scale-for-Children-III) assessed between 15-months and 12-years. Our results suggested that common variation in rs4307059 is associated with ASD-related phenotypes and support the role of rs4307059 as QTL for ASD. We aim to extend this approach and investigate the association between the SNP and ASD-related traits assessed in mothers. We therefore propose to genotype rs4307059 in the ALSPAC maternal sample.

Date proposal received: 
Monday, 21 September, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Monday, 21 September, 2009
Keywords: 
Autism, Genetics
Primary keyword: 

B875 - Program Project Analysis of the Placenta in Fetal Origins of Adolesecent Health Neurocognitive Pulmonary and Somatic - 20/09/2009

B number: 
B875
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Dawn Misra (Wayne State University, USA)
Co-applicants: 
Prof Debbie A Lawlor (University of Bristol, UK), Dr Carolyn Salafia (Wayne State University, USA), Prof John Henderson (University of Bristol, UK), Prof Jean Golding (University of Bristol, UK), Dr Jeremy Miles (United States Department of Agriculture (USDA), USA)
Title of project: 
Program Project: Analysis of the Placenta in Fetal Origins of Adolesecent Health: Neurocognitive, Pulmonary and Somatic.
Proposal summary: 

We propose a program project that will permit the processing and analysis of all consented ALSPAC placentas that have follow-up data on at least one of the three outcomes proposed for the program project, as described below.

The program project will include an Administrative Core (details to be provided by ALSPAC).

A "Placental Core" will be headed by Dr Craig Platt, Senior... at BRI, with Dr Carolyn Salafia as Co-PI at Placental Analytics LLC. Dr Platt will be responsible for supervision and quality control of placental examination, photography, tissue sampling, and sample processing to wax blocks, Placental Analytics LLC will be responsible for image analysis of placental photographs, and preparation of all histologic slides deemed essential to study goals including routine (hematoxylin and eosin) and special (e.g., immunohistochemistry) stains, and will digitize all such slides in a standard and accessible format. At the completion of the program project, all data extracted from photographs of placentas and digitized slide files, plus the digitized slide files, slides and remainders of wax tissue blocks will be returned to ALSPAC/BRI (?) as a permanent archive.

Program subprojects:

Neurocognitive: "Placental structure and pathology and neurocognitive outcome"

PI Prof Jean Golding, Co-Investigators D Misra, J Miles, C Platt, C Steer, C Salafia

We propose to test the hypothesis that detailed markers within the placenta are related to specific cognitive, behavioural and psychological outcomes from early childhood to school leaving (see Annex, below). A range of statistical approaches including structural equation modelling, a method well known to the psychometric literature, will be used to determine the predictive value of the placental measures in regard to the outcomes. Particular attention will be paid to ways in which the placenta varies in regard to factors that have been shown to be related to adverse neurocognitive outcomes such as influenza in mid-pregnancy, alcohol in early pregnancy, maternal anxiety, maternal prenatal levels of lead and mercury, and paternal omega-3 intake. We will also assess the ways in which placental variation is observed in placentas of children with minor congenital defects (minor congenital malformations are found in excess among children with many different neurocognitive outcomes).

All the neurocognitive outcomes will be available, but the minor congenital malformations will need to be identified from the neonatal clinical records - for which this grant would budget to be extracted from existing medical records.

The placenta provides two routes of analysis that we propose will account for variation in neurocognitive development/developmental trajectory:

  1. Placental branching growth is determined by the branching growth of its vascular tree. The "neurovascular hypothesis of disease" summarizes recent recognition that the gene families, growth factors and molecular signals that promote and maintain neuronal health are the same factors that promote and maintain vascular health. We propose that measurement of the placental vascular tree, in terms of 2D chorionic surface, 3D placental volume, chorionic surface vascular networks and finer architecture of the placental fetal and terminal villi correlate with fetal neuronal growth and health and will therefore account for variability in neurocognitive outcome/developmental trajectory.
  2. Placental pathologies of inflammation and injury patterns that may indicate oxidative stress can be identified in clinically abnormal and in clinically "normal" pregnancies. These pathologies are measured by histologic features identified in tissue sections of the placenta. We propose that measurement of these features will provide quantitative estimates of fetal exposures to inflammation and oxidative stress, via fetal effects on the developing brain, will account for variation in neurocognitive outcome/ developmental trajectory.

Pulmonary: "Placental structure and pathology and pulmonary outcome"

PI Dr John Henderson, Co-Investigators D Misra, J Miles, C Platt, C Salafia

There is evidence from cohort studies that have measured lung function sequentially from childhood that decrements of lung volumes (FEV1) and flows (FEF25-75 and maximal flows at 75%, 50% and 25% remaining FVC) may become established by early childhood and then 'track' into adult life. The effect of this would be to diminish peak measures of lung function prior to the natural decline in lung function with age, thus hitting a threshold at which disease becomes manifest at an earlier age. This paradigm is compatible with additional insults, such as smoking, leading to more rapid decline of lung function & exacerbating early life deficits. Thus, there may be additive effects of early life factors and later insults leading to diseases such as COPD.

Birth cohort studies that have measured lung function shortly after birth have demonstrated differences in measures of airflow associated with different patterns of symptom presentation in early childhood (e.g. transient early wheezing is associated with decrements in V'maxFRC shortly after birth (before symptoms are manifest) but regresses to the mean during the first years after birth whereas children who develop wheeze in later childhood have normal V'maxFRC in infancy but lower values of FEV1 in later childhood than never-wheezers) and with in-utero exposure to maternal tobacco smoke - children exposed in utero have lower values of lung function after birth and are more likely to develop wheeze and asthma during childhood.

Following the work of Barker and others, who demonstrated that adult respiratory diseases and lung function (FEV1) is associated with birth weight, there is considerable interest in the notion that fetal growth restraint is associated with abnormalities of airway development during a critical window of exposure leading to canalisation of effects that persist through the lifecourse (but may manifest only in the context of a second insult, e.g. smoking or following the decline in lung function measures with ageing).

Again, the placenta provides two routes of analysis that we propose will account for variation in pulmonary function:

  1. Placental trophoblast epithelial branching growth, determined by the branching growth of its vascular tree, parallels the branching of the pulmonary epithelium that is coordinated with the development of its parallel vascular tree. Major airways are laid down in the embryo/fetus at similar times as the major chorionic surface vessels, and more distal villous branching parallels later lung growth in utero. The close linkage of these two organ's development is highlighted by the difficulty in studying mutations of genes such as FGF and Notch that are key to lung development, as many of such muiutations are embryonic lethals because of failure of the development of the placenta. We propose that measurement of the placental vascular tree, in terms of 2D chorionic surface, 3D placental volume, chorionic surface vascular networks and finer architecture of the placental fetal and terminal villi correlate with fetal pulmonary growth and health and will therefore account for variability in pulmonary function.

  1. Placental pathologies of inflammation and injury patterns that may indicate oxidative stress can be identified in clinically abnormal and in clinically "normal" pregnancies. These pathologies are measured by histologic features identified in tissue sections of the placenta. Both inflammation, especially when cytokines are inhaled in the course of intraamniotic infections, and oxidative stress may alter pulmonary branching morphogenesis. We propose that measurement of these features will provide quantitative estimates of fetal exposures to inflammation and oxidative stress that will account for variability in pulmonary function.

More specifically, ALSPAC has detailed respiratory history at approximately 12 monthly intervals from birth and has measured lung function by spirometry at 8 and 15 years, allowing associations of both attained lung function pre- and (mostly) post-puberty and trajectory of lung function growth during adolescence (with a wealth of background information on exposures during this period, such as passive and active tobacco smoke exposure, validated with measurements of tobacco metabolites (cotinine).

Subproject 3 "Maternal overnutrition and offspring fat mass, metabolic and vascular function"

Principal Investigator: Dawn Misra, PhD, Co-Investigators: D Lawlor, J Miles, C Platt, C Salafia.

Dr. Lawlor's current grant aims refer to "offspring" generally; we have explicitly separated out the neonatal (birth outcomes) within our aims given the focus on the placenta.

SPECIFIC AIMS

Specific Aim 1: To investigate the influence of maternal BMI, weight gain, and diet during pregnancy on placental anthropometric measures. Specifically, we will assess and study the following placental parameters: placental weight, thickness, diameters; umbilical cord length; shape; symmetry measures; chorionic vascular branching pattern). The following null hypotheses will be tested:

1A. Maternal BMI does not explain variation in placental size and shape.

1B. Maternal weight gain does not explain variation in placental size and shape.

1C. Maternal diet does not explain variation in placental size and shape.

Specific Aim 2: To determine whether and how placental size and shape influences birth size. The following null hypotheses will be tested:

2A. Placental size and shape are not associated with birth size (e.g. weight, birth weight ratio, length, ponderal index).

2B. Associations between maternal factors and birth size are mediated by variation in placental size and shape.

Specific Aim 3: To determine whether and how placental size and shape influence offspring growth. Offspring growth was measured at multiple time points beginning at age 1 year up to 15 years of age and includes measures of adiposity (DXA assessed fat mass and fat distribution), vascular function (blood pressure, pulse pressure and endothelial function), and metabolic function (fasting glucose, insulin and lipids). The following null hypotheses will be tested:

3A. Placental size and shape are not associated with offspring childhood adiposity.

3B. Placental size and shape are not associated with offspring childhood vascular function.

3C. Placental size and shape are not associated with offspring childhood metabolic function.

3D. Associations between placental size and shape with offspring outcomes are mediated by effects of the placenta on birth size.

Specific Aim 4: To investigate the role of genetic variation on placental size and shape. DNA was extracted from mothers and offspring in the parent study. The following null hypotheses will be tested:

4A. Genetic variation does not explain variation in placental size and shape.

4B. Interactions between genes and the selected maternal factors (BMI, weight gain, diet) do not explain additional variations in placental size or shape.

Specific Aim 5: To develop and validate a placental index to identify offspring at risk for adverse outcomes with regard to adiposity, vascular function, and metabolic function.

Pilot analyses for the program subprojects will be generated using the placental data already collected from the Children in Focus subset (CIF, N=1050), and intermediate neurocognitive outcomes (TBD by Prof Golding), intermediate pulmonary function outcomes (TBD by Dr Henderson), and BMI, waist & fat & lean mass (DXA) at age 9 as previously determined by Dr Lawlor. These pilot analyses will provide strong support for our (generally well received) October submission, and we are optimistic about receiving funding based on our July resubmission. Dawn Misra and Jeremy Miles will work together on data analysis for the 3 subprojects, with Colin Steer additionally on the neurocognitive subproject.

Annex

1. Temperament and behaviour of child: Infant temperament using the Carey

at ages 6 and 24 months; EAS temperament scale at 42 months;

Behaviour measures of hyperactivity, antisocial/conduct problems, peer

problems, emotional behaviour and prosocial behaviour as well as overall

behaviour difficulties, measured at ages 4 - 13 years

2. Child development: mental and motor development measured annually

between 6 months and 4 years

3. Cognitive measures: IQ using the WISC at age 8; short term memory;

executive function;

4. Speech and language: Vocabulary measured from 15 months to 3 years;

Grammatical understanding; Phonological problems; Use of speech

tharapist

5. Motor coordination: Balance tests; fine motor coordination; Gross motor

coordination; Functional motor impairment, including DCD

6. Childhood growth: Height; Weight; Head circumference; Hip

circumference; Waist circumference; Body mass index - all at various time

points from 4 months to 15 years

7. Educational attainments: Results of SATS tests at 7, 11 and 14; GCSE

results; Results of ALSPAC's own tests ofReading, Spelling, Mathematics,

and Science understanding.

8. Attention and hyperactivity: Hyperactive and inattentive behaviour using

questions to parents and teachers from 4 to 11 years and to the children

at 13.

9. Hearing ability: Tests of hearing under standardised conditions at ages 7,

9 and 11 - using audiometry and otoacoustic emissions; Hearing of

speech discrimination at 3.5 and 5 years

10. Visual ability: Tests of visual acuity, stereoacuity, and other features of

visual ability at various ages from 8 months to 11 years

11. Other sensory outcomes (taste and smell): Taste test (PROP) results at

age 10; Smell test results at age 11

12. Childhood psychiatric disorders: Autism; ADHD; ADD; Anorexia nervosa;

Bulimia; Depression; Anxiety disorders; Conduct disorder;

Date proposal received: 
Sunday, 20 September, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Sunday, 20 September, 2009
Keywords: 
Placenta
Primary keyword: 

B877 - Do associations between adolescent mental health and self-harm differ in males and females - 18/09/2009

B number: 
B877
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Emily Klineberg (University of Bristol, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Prof David Gunnell (University of Bristol, UK), Dr Judi Kidger (University of Bristol, UK)
Title of project: 
Do associations between adolescent mental health and self-harm differ in males and females?
Proposal summary: 

Background:

Community-based research indicates that over 10% of adolescents aged 15-16 have self-harmed at some point their lives (1). As only a small proportion of young people who self-harm seek formal help, further research is required at a population level to explore potentially modifiable factors associated with this behaviour (1,2). There has been limited prospective research predicting adolescent self-harm in community samples, particularly in the UK (3-7). Functions of self-harm vary between people, and over time; including being a short-term release of passing distress, or an aspect of an enduring mental illness.

Self-harm is a strong predictor of later suicide (8-9) and further self-harm (10-11). The prevalence of self-harm varies by gender in adolescents, with substantially higher rates in girls (1-6). For example, a cross-sectional school-based survey in Oxfordshire reported that 11.2% of females and 3.2% of males aged 15-16 years had self-harmed in the previous year (1). This contrasts with the incidence of suicide being 3-4 times higher in males complared with females. The relationship between self-injurious behaviour and gender is not entirely clear. Self-harm may serve a different function for young males and females.

Analysis included in EK's recently submitted PhD examined data from longitudinal school-based study examining risk and protective factors for self-harm in a sample of 1023 East London adolescents (Research with East London Adolescents; Community Health Survey; www.relachs.org). Cross-sectional associations between self-harm and sub-clinical psychological distress, such as borderline scores on the SDQ, and longitudinal associations between self-harm, conduct problems (SDQ) and depressive symptoms (assessed with the Short Moods and Feelings Questionnaire), illustrated that self-harm may relate to different aspects of mental health, as well as different levels of symptom severity.

There was some evidence for a gender difference in the relationships between mental health and self-harm, with an association between previous depressive symptoms and self-harm in boys (OR 6.40, 95%CI 2.14-20.60, p=0.01), which was not evident in girls. The sample size limited the analysis and there was insufficient power to explore this further, thus research with a larger sample is required. Gaining more insight into whether adolescent self-harm has different implications in young males and females has potential to inform targeted interventions and responses to self-harm. Associations between self-harm and sub-threshold psychological distress may imply that interventions need to focus on general emotional health and well-being, not necessarily the recognition of and response to diagnosable mental health problems. The proposed analysis would examine gender differences in relationships between mental health in early adolescence (11-13 years) and self-harm at age 16.

Proposed analysis:

This proposal will constitute part of an application (by EK, mentored by DG and JK) for a 1 year ESRC postdoctoral fellowship, in which a limited amount of additional research related to PhD may be undertaken in addition to writing papers for publication from doctoral research.

The proposed analysis will aim to examine data already collected as part of ALSPAC, addressing two main research questions:

(i) Which aspects of mental health measured at ages 11 and 13 have prospective associations with self-harm at age 16?

(ii) Are there gender differences in the associations between mental health and self-harm?

Multivariable logistic regression would be used to examine associations with self-harm reported at age 16+, by gender. Potential predictors would include self-harm at age 11, assessed as a part of Borderline Personality Disorder, and subscales from psychological measures (DAWBA and SDQ) assessing depressive symptoms, emotional problems, conduct problems and oppositional behaviour at ages 11 and 13. Additionally, demographic data, and personality factors such as sensation seeking, locus of control and self-concept may be relevant to consider.

References:

1. Hawton, K. et al (2002) 'Deliberate self harm in adolescents: self report survey in schools in England', British Medical Journal, 325; 1207-11.

2. Fortune, S. et al (2008) 'Help seeking before and after episodes of self-harm: a descriptive study in school pupils in England', BMC Public Health, 8:369.

3. Evans, E. et al (2004) 'Factors associated with suicidal phenomena in adolescents: A systematic review of population-based studies', Clinical Psychology Review, 24:957-79.

4. Patton, G. et al (1997) 'Adolescent suicidal behaviours: a population-based study of risk', Psychological Medicine, 27: 715-24.

5. Young, R. et al (2006) 'Prevalence of deliberate self-harm and attempted suicide within contemporary Goth youth subculture: longitudinal cohort study', British Medical Journal, 332: 1058-61.

6. Martin, G. et al (2005) 'Perceived academic performance, self-esteem and locus of control as indicators of need for assessment of adolescent suicide risk: implications for teachers', Journal of Adolescence, 28:75-87.

7. Haavisto, A. et al (2005) 'Factors associated with ideation and acts of deliberate self-harm among 18 year old boys', Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 40:912-21.

8. Hawton, K. et al (1999) 'Suicide in young people: a study of 174 cases, aged under 25 years, based on coroners' and medical records', British Journal of Psychiatry, 175:1-6.

9. Cooper, J. et al (2005) 'Suicide After Deliberate Self-Harm: A 4-Year Cohort Study', American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(2):297-303.

10. Hawton, K. et al (2000) 'Deliberate self-harm in adolescents in Oxford, 1985-1995', Journal of Adolescence, 23: 47-55.

11. Hawton, K. et al (2003) 'Deliberate self-harm in adolescents: a study of characteristics and trends in Oxford, 1990-2000', Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(8): 1191-1198.

Date proposal received: 
Friday, 18 September, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Friday, 18 September, 2009
Keywords: 
Mental Health, Self-harm
Primary keyword: 

Pages