Proposal summaries

These are research proposals that have been approved by the ALSPAC exec. The titles include a B number which identifies the proposal and the date on which the proposals received ALSPAC exec approval.

Click here to export results in Word format.

B878 - Psychosocial Stress Growth Tempo and Pubertal Timing - 23/09/2009

B number: 
B878
Principal applicant name: 
Prof Ren?e Danielle Boynton-Jarrett (Boston University, USA)
Co-applicants: 
Dr Michele Marcus (Emory University, USA), Dr Mildred Maisonet (Emory University, USA)
Title of project: 
Psychosocial Stress, Growth Tempo, and Pubertal Timing.
Proposal summary: 

A building body of research evidence suggests that intrauterine and early life exposures contribute to chronic disease risk in later life. While recent evidence supports the role of childhood adversities on reproductive lifespan,1-4 yet there is a limited understanding of mechanisms through which these adversities impact health. Researchers have established an association between psychosocial stressors in childhood and disruption of pubertal timing. The strongest literature is among girls, and using age at menarche (AAM) as a proxy measure for pubertal timing. Earlier age at menarche has been linked to all cause mortality,5 risk for breast cancer,6-8 cardiovascular disease,9 depression,10 metabolic syndrome11 and associated features, including overweight/obesity,12,13 insulin resistance,14 and polycystic ovarian syndrome,10 while later age at menarche is associated with depression,15,16 fractures,17-19 and lower bone mineral density.20-22 To date there has been limited exploration of psychosocial predictors of pubertal onset and the tempo of pubertal development among both girls and boys.

Psychosocial antecedents of age of menarche include: familial conflict,23 alterations in family structure,24 stressful home circumstances,25-27 paternal absence in childhood,28 and quality of familial relationships.29,30 3,25,31-34 A growing number of studies have demonstrated an association between child abuse and age at menarche.3,35-39 In the five studies to date sexual abuse has demonstrated a stronger and more consistent association with early menarche while physical abuse has had a weakly positive association in some studies.3,35-39 A shared limitation of these studies is the failure to account for additional childhood hardships, such as poverty, that are also associated with pubertal development.

The intent of this study is to investigate the independent and cumulative impact of multiple types of childhood adversities (including child abuse, neglect, family conflict, and socioeconomic status) on timing of puberty in this prospective birth cohort. We hypothesized that children exposed to higher cumulative adversities will be more likely to have altered timing of menarche-earlier or later age at onset. We proposed to investigate the relation between exposure to early life adversities and the tempo of pubertal development and timing of puberty. Specifically, we are interested in exploring the impact of exposure to physical abuse, sexual abuse, emotional abuse, neglect, and/or family conflict and the timing and tempo of pubertal development. We hypothesize that childhood adversities may impact pubertal development through influences on rate of change in BMI over time, lifestyles and health behaviours, including dietary habits, physical activity, and sedentary behaviours. Existing studies are limited by retrospective exposure assessment, and failure to consider multiple types of adversities.

Thus, a proposed mechanism linking exposure to child abuse and sexual maturation is through dysregulation of neuroendocrine systems that respond to environmental challenges (such as the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA), hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal (HPG) axes. Child maltreatment is a significant psychosocial stressor and therefore may influence responsiveness of the HPA and HPG axes, neuroendocrine functioning, hormonal milieu and ultimately influence timing of sexual maturation. In addition, child abuse may be associated with nutritional factors, energy expenditure, level of activity, and health behaviors in early life that impact AAM. Compelling evidence from several recent studies suggest a relation between childhood adversities and body mass index (BMI) in adulthood.40,41 Child maltreatment has been linked to both excessive weight control and obesity,40-47 and disordered eating.48-53

A second area of interest is the relation between pubertal development and disordered eating patterns and body dissatisfaction. Puberty is one commonly cited risk factor for disordered eating, body dissatisfaction, and eating disorders.10,54 Past studies which have investigated the timing of puberty in relation to the development of eating disorders, and body dissatisfaction have produced mixed findings.54-58 A common limitation shared by previous studies includes the use of cross-sectional methodology, clinical samples, or small community samples. The advantage of the AVON study is that it is robust and comprehensive in the measurement of disordered eating and puberty, the longitudinal methodology, sample size, and sample population. This secondary analysis will focus on the impact of characteristics of pubertal development, particularly timing and tempo, on patterns of disordered eating, eating disorders, and degree of body dissatisfaction.

Please see the attached Excel spreadsheet which outlines the concepts, measures, source, and time points requested (as outlined in application item #8).

References

1. Mishra GD, Cooper R, Tom SE, Kuh D. Early life circumstances and their impact on menarche and menopause. Womens Health (Lond Engl) 2009;5(2):175-90.

2. Parent AS, Teilmann G, Juul A, Skakkebaek NE, Toppari J, Bourguignon JP. The timing of normal puberty and the age limits of sexual precocity: variations around the world, secular trends, and changes after migration. Endocr Rev 2003;24(5):668-93.

3. Romans SE, Martin JM, Gendall K, Herbison GP. Age of menarche: the role of some psychosocial factors. Psychol Med 2003;33(5):933-9.

4. Slyper AH. The pubertal timing controversy in the USA, and a review of possible causative factors for the advance in timing of onset of puberty. Clin Endocrinol (Oxf) 2006;65(1):1-8.

5. Jacobsen BK, Heuch I, Kvale G. Association of low age at menarche with increased all-cause mortality: a 37-year follow-up of 61,319 Norwegian women. Am J Epidemiol 2007;166(12):1431-7.

6. Hamilton AS, Mack TM. Puberty and genetic susceptibility to breast cancer in a case-control study in twins. N Engl J Med 2003;348(23):2313-22.

7. Bernstein L. Epidemiology of endocrine-related risk factors for breast cancer. J Mammary Gland Biol Neoplasia 2002;7(1):3-15.

8. Romundstad PR, Vatten LJ, Nilsen TI, Holmen TL, Hsieh CC, Trichopoulos D, Stuver SO. Birth size in relation to age at menarche and adolescent body size: implications for breast cancer risk. Int J Cancer 2003;105(3):400-3.

9. Remsberg KE, Demerath EW, Schubert CM, Chumlea WC, Sun SS, Siervogel RM. Early menarche and the development of cardiovascular disease risk factors in adolescent girls: the Fels Longitudinal Study. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2005;90(5):2718-24.

10. Stice E, Presnell K, Bearman SK. Relation of early menarche to depression, eating disorders, substance abuse, and comorbid psychopathology among adolescent girls. Dev Psychol 2001;37(5):608-19.

11. Frontini MG, Srinivasan SR, Berenson GS. Longitudinal changes in risk variables underlying metabolic Syndrome X from childhood to young adulthood in female subjects with a history of early menarche: the Bogalusa Heart Study.Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 2003;27(11):1398-404.

12. Kaplowitz PB, Slora EJ, Wasserman RC, Pedlow SE, Herman-Giddens ME.Earlier onset of puberty in girls: relation to increased body mass index and race. Pediatrics 2001;108(2):347-53.

13. Adair LS, Gordon-Larsen P. Maturational timing and overweight prevalence in US adolescent girls. Am J Public Health 2001;91(4):642-4.

14. Ibanez L, Potau N, de Zegher F. Recognition of a new association: reduced fetal growth, precocious pubarche, hyperinsulinism and ovarian dysfunction. Ann Endocrinol (Paris) 2000;61(2):141-2.

15. Bisaga K, Petkova E, Cheng J, Davies M, Feldman JF, Whitaker AH. Menstrual functioning and psychopathology in a county-wide population of high school girls. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2002;41(10):1197-204.

16. Herva A, Jokelainen J, Pouta A, Veijola J, Timonen M, Karvonen JT, Joukamaa M. Age at menarche and depression at the age of 31 years: findings from the Northern Finland 1966 Birth Cohort Study. J Psychosom Res 2004;57(4):359-62.

17. Fujiwara S, Kasagi F, Yamada M, Kodama K. Risk factors for hip fracture in a Japanese cohort. J Bone Miner Res 1997;12(7):998-1004.

18. Roy DK, O'Neill TW, Finn JD, Lunt M, Silman AJ, Felsenberg D, Armbrecht G, Banzer D, Benevolenskaya LI, Bhalla A, Bruges Armas J, Cannata JB, Cooper C, Dequeker J, Diaz MN, Eastell R, Yershova OB, Felsch B, Gowin W, Havelka S, Hoszowski K, Ismail AA, Jajic I, Janott I, Johnell O, Kanis JA, Kragl G, Lopez Vaz A, Lorenc R, Lyritis G, Masaryk P, Matthis C, Miazgowski T, Gennari C, Pols HA, Poor G, Raspe HH, Reid DM, Reisinger W, Scheidt-Nave C, Stepan JJ, Todd CJ, Weber K, Woolf AD, Reeve J. Determinants of incident vertebral fracture in men and women: results from the European Prospective Osteoporosis Study (EPOS). Osteoporos Int 2003;14(1):19-26.

19. Silman AJ. Risk factors for Colles' fracture in men and women: results from the European Prospective Osteoporosis Study. Osteoporos Int 2003;14(3):213-8.

20. Rosenthal DI, Mayo-Smith W, Hayes CW, Khurana JS, Biller BM, Neer RM, Klibanski A. Age and bone mass in premenopausal women. J Bone Miner Res 1989;4(4):533-8.

21. Varenna M, Binelli L, Zucchi F, Ghiringhelli D, Gallazzi M, Sinigaglia L. Prevalence of osteoporosis by educational level in a cohort of postmenopausal women. Osteoporos Int 1999;9(3):236-41.

22. Eastell R. Role of oestrogen in the regulation of bone turnover at the menarche. J Endocrinol 2005;185(2):223-34.

23. Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Belsky J, Silva PA. Childhood experience and the onset of menarche: a test of a sociobiological model. Child Dev 1992;63(1):47-58.

24. Pesonen AK, Raikkonen K, Heinonen K, Kajantie E, Forsen T, Eriksson JG. Reproductive traits following a parent-child separation trauma during childhood: a natural experiment during World War II. Am J Hum Biol 2008;20(3):345-51.

25. Wierson M, Long PJ, Forehand RL. Toward a new understanding of early menarche: the role of environmental stress in pubertal timing. Adolescence 1993;28(112):913-24.

26. Ellis BJ, Essex MJ. Family environments, adrenarche, and sexual maturation: a longitudinal test of a life history model. Child Dev 2007;78(6):1799-817.

27. Bogaert AF. Age at puberty and father absence in a national probability sample. J Adolesc 2005;28(4):541-6.

28. Belsky J, Steinberg L, Draper P. Childhood experience, interpersonal development, and reproductive strategy: and evolutionary theory of socialization. Child Dev 1991;62(4):647-70.

29. Ellis BJ, Garber J. Psychosocial antecedents of variation in girls' pubertal timing: maternal depression, stepfather presence, and marital and family stress. Child Dev 2000;71(2):485-501.

30. Ellis BJ, McFadyen-Ketchum S, Dodge KA, Pettit GS, Bates JE. Quality of early family relationships and individual differences in the timing of pubertal maturation in girls: a longitudinal test of an evolutionary model. J Pers Soc Psychol 1999;77(2):387-401.

31. Maestripieri D, Roney JR, DeBias N, Durante KM, Spaepen GM. Father absence, menarche and interest in infants among adolescent girls. Dev Sci 2004;7(5):560-6.

32. Saxbe DE, Repetti RL. Brief report: Fathers' and mothers' marital relationship predicts daughters' pubertal development two years later. J Adolesc 2008.

33. Tither JM, Ellis BJ. Impact of fathers on daughters' age at menarche: a genetically and environmentally controlled sibling study. Dev Psychol 2008;44(5):1409-20.

34. Mendle J, Turkheimer E, D'Onofrio BM, Lynch SK, Emery RE, Slutske WS, Martin NG. Family structure and age at menarche: a children-of-twins approach. Dev Psychol 2006;42(3):533-42.

35. Brown J, Cohen P, Chen H, Smailes E, Johnson JG. Sexual trajectories of abused and neglected youths. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2004;25(2):77-82.

36. Foster H, Hagan J, Brooks-Gunn J. Growing up fast: stress exposure and subjective "weathering" in emerging adulthood. J Health Soc Behav 2008;49(2):162-77.

37. Vigil JM, Geary DC, Byrd-Craven J. A life history assessment of early childhood sexual abuse in women. Dev Psychol 2005;41(3):553-61.

38. Wise LA, Palmer JR, Rothman EF, Rosenberg L. Childhood Abuse and Early Menarche: Findings From the Black Women's Health Study. Am J Public Health 2009.

39. Zabin LS, Emerson MR, Rowland DL. Childhood sexual abuse and early menarche: the direction of their relationship and its implications. J Adolesc Health 2005;36(5):393-400.

40. Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, Williamson DF, Spitz AM, Edwards V, Koss MP, Marks JS. Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults. The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. Am J Prev Med 1998;14(4):245-58.

41. Williamson DF, Thompson TJ, Anda RF, Dietz WH, Felitti V. Body weight and obesity in adults and self-reported abuse in childhood.Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 2002;26(8):1075-82.

42. Lissau I, Sorensen TI.Parental neglect during childhood and increased risk of obesity in young adulthood. Lancet 1994;343(8893):324-7.

43. Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, Williamson DF, Spitz AM, Edwards V, Koss MP, Marks JS. Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults. The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. Am J Prev Med 1998;14(4):245-58.

44. Gunstad J, Paul RH, Spitznagel MB, Cohen RA, Williams LM, Kohn M, Gordon E. Exposure to early life trauma is associated with adult obesity. Psychiatry Res 2006;142(1):31-7.

45. Lissau I, Sorensen TI. Parental neglect during childhood and increased risk of obesity in young adulthood. Lancet 1994;343(8893):324-7.

46. Noll JG, Zeller MH, Trickett PK, Putnam FW. Obesity risk for female victims of childhood sexual abuse: a prospective study. Pediatrics 2007;120(1):e61-7.

47. Williamson DF, Thompson TJ, Anda RF, Dietz WH, Felitti V. Body weight and obesity in adults and self-reported abuse in childhood. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 2002;26(8):1075-82.

48. Johnson J, Cohen P, Kasen S, Brook JS. Childhood adversities associated with risk for eating disorders or weight problems during adolescence or early adulthood. American Journal of Psychiatry 2002;159(3):394-400.

49. Wonderlich SA, Wilsnack RW, Wilsnack SC, Harris TR. Childhood sexual abuse and bulimic behavior in a nationally representative sample. Am J Public Health 1996;86:1082-1086.

50. Everill JT, Waller G. Reported sexual abuse and eating psychopathology: a review of the evidence for a causal link. Int J Eat Disord. 1995;18:1-11.

51. Welch SL, Fairburn CG. Childhood sexual and physical abuse as risk factors for the development of bulimia nervosa. Child Abuse Negl. 1996;20:633-642.

52. Ackard DM, Neumark-Sztainer D, Hannan PJ, French S, Story M. Binge and purge behavior among adolescents: associations with sexual and physical abuse in a nationally representative sample: the Commonwealth Fund survey. Child Abuse Negl 2001;25(6):771-85.

53. Fonseca H, Ireland M, Resnick MD. Familial correlates of extreme weight control behaviors among adolescents. Int J Eat Disord 2002;32(4):441-8.

54. Patton GC, Viner R. Pubertal transitions in health. Lancet 2007;369(9567):1130-9.

55. Ackard DM, Peterson CB. Association between puberty and disordered eating, body image, and other psychological variables. Int J Eat Disord 2001;29(2):187-94.

56. Kaltiala-Heino R, Rimpela M, Rissanen A, Rantanen P. Early puberty and early sexual activity are associated with bulimic-type eating pathology in middle adolescence. J Adolesc Health 2001;28(4):346-52.

57. Killen JD, Hayward C, Litt I, Hammer LD, Wilson DM, Miner B, Taylor CB, Varady A, Shisslak C. Is puberty a risk factor for eating disorders? Am J Dis Child 1992;146(3):323-5.

58. Mangweth-Matzek B, Rupp CI, Hausmann A, Kemmler G, Biebl W. Menarche, puberty, and first sexual activities in eating-disordered patients as compared with a psychiatric and a nonpsychiatric control group. Int J Eat Disord 2007;40(8):705-10.

Date proposal received: 
Wednesday, 23 September, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Wednesday, 23 September, 2009
Keywords: 
Growth, Puberty, Stress
Primary keyword: 

B882 - Pathways to Alcohol Us Disorders in ALSPAC A Genetic-Developmental Study - 22/09/2009

B number: 
B882
Principal applicant name: 
Prof Kenneth Kendler (Virginia Commonwealth University, USA)
Co-applicants: 
Dr Danielle Dick (Virginia Commonwealth University, USA), Dr Alexis Edwards (Virginia Commonwealth University, USA)
Title of project: 
Pathways to Alcohol Us Disorders in ALSPAC: A Genetic-Developmental Study.
Proposal summary: 

There is considerable evidence suggesting that a wide range of childhood and adolescent externalizing behaviors are prospectively associated with the subsequent development of alcohol use (AU) and alcohol use problems (AUPs), including several dimensions on which data have been collected within the ALSPAC study: e.g., conduct problems, temperament, personality, sensation seeking, and impulsivity. Clearly, we would not expect these various aspects of externalizing behavior to act independently, but rather to operate on AU and AUPs through a dynamic system of influences, both within and across time. For example, early temperament has been shown to influence later conduct problems, for which there are also known associations with aspects of personality, impulsivity, and sensation seeking. There is little clarity, however, regarding how mechanistic associations between these facets ultimately influence AU and AUPs in adolescence and adulthood. Prior to examining the structural nature of these relationships, we must first establish the factor structures within each of the dimensions, independently. So, for example, ALSPAC has collected information on conduct problems assessed across several points in time, and with multiple instruments (e.g., the Revised Rutter Parent Scale for Preschool Children at 42 months and repeated assessments of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire at ages 6.5, 9.5, and 11.5 years). In order to assess development within conduct problems, we must first confirm that the two scales tap into the same underlying construct. Toward this end, one strategy that we propose is to identify a set of "anchor" items/behaviors that are assessed within both measures (e.g., is inconsiderate of others, steals, destroys/damages property, fights, bullies, lies/cheats). We can then subject this common set of variables to within-wave confirmatory factor analysis, allowing us to differentiate between items that yield consistent parameter estimates across time (equivalent factor loadings across assessments) and those for which associations with the underlying construct vary across time (i.e., some items may reflect behavior that is more or less indicative of conduct problems at different stages of development). Time-specific factor score estimates (FSEs) can then be derived by specifying a model which equates factor loadings across time for those items displaying factorial invariance, while freely estimating loadings at each wave of assessment for those items displaying developmental inconsistency. Moreover, we propose to implement these procedures across the different dimensions of externalizing behavior that have been conducted in ALSPAC. The resulting FSEs, which capitalize on consistency while also recognizing the potential for specific behaviors to increase/decrease in relevance across development, can then be used as the unit of measurement in models addressing the complex structural relationships between individual dimensions of behavior, general domains of risk, and alcohol-related outcomes. Accordingly, the broad goal of this set of analyses will be to characterize the measurement of, and inter-relationships between, different domains of externalizing behavior that have been assessed in ALSPAC, in order to use this information to study how different facets of externalizing behavior relate to alcohol use. We have previously had a conference call with Barbara Maughan (and Glyn Lewis) and discussed her interesting work on trajectories of conduct problems and externalizing behavior in the ALSPAC sample, so we would plan to work with her (and other interested ALSPAC collaborators) on these efforts, so as to ensure there is no duplication of effort across the projects. Since our primary interest is in understanding how externalizing behavior relates to alcohol use (with the latter being our primary outcome of interest), and her interest is in the nature of externalizing behavior itself (her primary outcome of interest), we believe that our projects will be synergistic rather than overlapping in scope.

Date proposal received: 
Tuesday, 22 September, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Tuesday, 22 September, 2009
Keywords: 
Alcohol
Primary keyword: 

B876 - Association analysis between a high risk autism SNP and the broader autism phenotype in the ALSPAC cohort - 21/09/2009

B number: 
B876
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Beate St. Pourcain (University of Bristol, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Prof George Davey Smith (University of Bristol, UK)
Title of project: 
Association analysis between a high risk autism SNP and the broader autism phenotype in the ALSPAC cohort.
Proposal summary: 

Recent genome-wide analysis identified a genetic variant on 5p14.1 (rs4307059), which is associated with risk for autism spectrum disorder (ASD). We recently investigated up to 7313 ALSPAC children and studied association between rs4307059 and potential broader autism traits including early vocabulary and communication (MacArthur-Communicative-Development-Inventory/MacArthur-Toddler-Communication-Questionnaire; Denver-Developmental-Screening-Test), social behaviour and behavioural difficulties (Emotionality-Activity-Sociability-Temperament-Survey; Revised-Rutter-Parent-Scale for Preschool Children; Standard-Assessment-Tests of social skills; Strengths-and-Difficulties-Questionnaire, Cambridge Hormones-and-Moods-Project-Friendship-Questionnaire; Special-Educational-Needs assessment), social communication (Social-and-Communications-Disorders-Checklist; Children's-Communication-Checklist) and verbal intelligence (Wechsler-Intelligence-Scale-for-Children-III) assessed between 15-months and 12-years. Our results suggested that common variation in rs4307059 is associated with ASD-related phenotypes and support the role of rs4307059 as QTL for ASD. We aim to extend this approach and investigate the association between the SNP and ASD-related traits assessed in mothers. We therefore propose to genotype rs4307059 in the ALSPAC maternal sample.

Date proposal received: 
Monday, 21 September, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Monday, 21 September, 2009
Keywords: 
Autism, Genetics
Primary keyword: 

B875 - Program Project Analysis of the Placenta in Fetal Origins of Adolesecent Health Neurocognitive Pulmonary and Somatic - 20/09/2009

B number: 
B875
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Dawn Misra (Wayne State University, USA)
Co-applicants: 
Prof Debbie A Lawlor (University of Bristol, UK), Dr Carolyn Salafia (Wayne State University, USA), Prof John Henderson (University of Bristol, UK), Prof Jean Golding (University of Bristol, UK), Dr Jeremy Miles (United States Department of Agriculture (USDA), USA)
Title of project: 
Program Project: Analysis of the Placenta in Fetal Origins of Adolesecent Health: Neurocognitive, Pulmonary and Somatic.
Proposal summary: 

We propose a program project that will permit the processing and analysis of all consented ALSPAC placentas that have follow-up data on at least one of the three outcomes proposed for the program project, as described below.

The program project will include an Administrative Core (details to be provided by ALSPAC).

A "Placental Core" will be headed by Dr Craig Platt, Senior... at BRI, with Dr Carolyn Salafia as Co-PI at Placental Analytics LLC. Dr Platt will be responsible for supervision and quality control of placental examination, photography, tissue sampling, and sample processing to wax blocks, Placental Analytics LLC will be responsible for image analysis of placental photographs, and preparation of all histologic slides deemed essential to study goals including routine (hematoxylin and eosin) and special (e.g., immunohistochemistry) stains, and will digitize all such slides in a standard and accessible format. At the completion of the program project, all data extracted from photographs of placentas and digitized slide files, plus the digitized slide files, slides and remainders of wax tissue blocks will be returned to ALSPAC/BRI (?) as a permanent archive.

Program subprojects:

Neurocognitive: "Placental structure and pathology and neurocognitive outcome"

PI Prof Jean Golding, Co-Investigators D Misra, J Miles, C Platt, C Steer, C Salafia

We propose to test the hypothesis that detailed markers within the placenta are related to specific cognitive, behavioural and psychological outcomes from early childhood to school leaving (see Annex, below). A range of statistical approaches including structural equation modelling, a method well known to the psychometric literature, will be used to determine the predictive value of the placental measures in regard to the outcomes. Particular attention will be paid to ways in which the placenta varies in regard to factors that have been shown to be related to adverse neurocognitive outcomes such as influenza in mid-pregnancy, alcohol in early pregnancy, maternal anxiety, maternal prenatal levels of lead and mercury, and paternal omega-3 intake. We will also assess the ways in which placental variation is observed in placentas of children with minor congenital defects (minor congenital malformations are found in excess among children with many different neurocognitive outcomes).

All the neurocognitive outcomes will be available, but the minor congenital malformations will need to be identified from the neonatal clinical records - for which this grant would budget to be extracted from existing medical records.

The placenta provides two routes of analysis that we propose will account for variation in neurocognitive development/developmental trajectory:

  1. Placental branching growth is determined by the branching growth of its vascular tree. The "neurovascular hypothesis of disease" summarizes recent recognition that the gene families, growth factors and molecular signals that promote and maintain neuronal health are the same factors that promote and maintain vascular health. We propose that measurement of the placental vascular tree, in terms of 2D chorionic surface, 3D placental volume, chorionic surface vascular networks and finer architecture of the placental fetal and terminal villi correlate with fetal neuronal growth and health and will therefore account for variability in neurocognitive outcome/developmental trajectory.
  2. Placental pathologies of inflammation and injury patterns that may indicate oxidative stress can be identified in clinically abnormal and in clinically "normal" pregnancies. These pathologies are measured by histologic features identified in tissue sections of the placenta. We propose that measurement of these features will provide quantitative estimates of fetal exposures to inflammation and oxidative stress, via fetal effects on the developing brain, will account for variation in neurocognitive outcome/ developmental trajectory.

Pulmonary: "Placental structure and pathology and pulmonary outcome"

PI Dr John Henderson, Co-Investigators D Misra, J Miles, C Platt, C Salafia

There is evidence from cohort studies that have measured lung function sequentially from childhood that decrements of lung volumes (FEV1) and flows (FEF25-75 and maximal flows at 75%, 50% and 25% remaining FVC) may become established by early childhood and then 'track' into adult life. The effect of this would be to diminish peak measures of lung function prior to the natural decline in lung function with age, thus hitting a threshold at which disease becomes manifest at an earlier age. This paradigm is compatible with additional insults, such as smoking, leading to more rapid decline of lung function & exacerbating early life deficits. Thus, there may be additive effects of early life factors and later insults leading to diseases such as COPD.

Birth cohort studies that have measured lung function shortly after birth have demonstrated differences in measures of airflow associated with different patterns of symptom presentation in early childhood (e.g. transient early wheezing is associated with decrements in V'maxFRC shortly after birth (before symptoms are manifest) but regresses to the mean during the first years after birth whereas children who develop wheeze in later childhood have normal V'maxFRC in infancy but lower values of FEV1 in later childhood than never-wheezers) and with in-utero exposure to maternal tobacco smoke - children exposed in utero have lower values of lung function after birth and are more likely to develop wheeze and asthma during childhood.

Following the work of Barker and others, who demonstrated that adult respiratory diseases and lung function (FEV1) is associated with birth weight, there is considerable interest in the notion that fetal growth restraint is associated with abnormalities of airway development during a critical window of exposure leading to canalisation of effects that persist through the lifecourse (but may manifest only in the context of a second insult, e.g. smoking or following the decline in lung function measures with ageing).

Again, the placenta provides two routes of analysis that we propose will account for variation in pulmonary function:

  1. Placental trophoblast epithelial branching growth, determined by the branching growth of its vascular tree, parallels the branching of the pulmonary epithelium that is coordinated with the development of its parallel vascular tree. Major airways are laid down in the embryo/fetus at similar times as the major chorionic surface vessels, and more distal villous branching parallels later lung growth in utero. The close linkage of these two organ's development is highlighted by the difficulty in studying mutations of genes such as FGF and Notch that are key to lung development, as many of such muiutations are embryonic lethals because of failure of the development of the placenta. We propose that measurement of the placental vascular tree, in terms of 2D chorionic surface, 3D placental volume, chorionic surface vascular networks and finer architecture of the placental fetal and terminal villi correlate with fetal pulmonary growth and health and will therefore account for variability in pulmonary function.

  1. Placental pathologies of inflammation and injury patterns that may indicate oxidative stress can be identified in clinically abnormal and in clinically "normal" pregnancies. These pathologies are measured by histologic features identified in tissue sections of the placenta. Both inflammation, especially when cytokines are inhaled in the course of intraamniotic infections, and oxidative stress may alter pulmonary branching morphogenesis. We propose that measurement of these features will provide quantitative estimates of fetal exposures to inflammation and oxidative stress that will account for variability in pulmonary function.

More specifically, ALSPAC has detailed respiratory history at approximately 12 monthly intervals from birth and has measured lung function by spirometry at 8 and 15 years, allowing associations of both attained lung function pre- and (mostly) post-puberty and trajectory of lung function growth during adolescence (with a wealth of background information on exposures during this period, such as passive and active tobacco smoke exposure, validated with measurements of tobacco metabolites (cotinine).

Subproject 3 "Maternal overnutrition and offspring fat mass, metabolic and vascular function"

Principal Investigator: Dawn Misra, PhD, Co-Investigators: D Lawlor, J Miles, C Platt, C Salafia.

Dr. Lawlor's current grant aims refer to "offspring" generally; we have explicitly separated out the neonatal (birth outcomes) within our aims given the focus on the placenta.

SPECIFIC AIMS

Specific Aim 1: To investigate the influence of maternal BMI, weight gain, and diet during pregnancy on placental anthropometric measures. Specifically, we will assess and study the following placental parameters: placental weight, thickness, diameters; umbilical cord length; shape; symmetry measures; chorionic vascular branching pattern). The following null hypotheses will be tested:

1A. Maternal BMI does not explain variation in placental size and shape.

1B. Maternal weight gain does not explain variation in placental size and shape.

1C. Maternal diet does not explain variation in placental size and shape.

Specific Aim 2: To determine whether and how placental size and shape influences birth size. The following null hypotheses will be tested:

2A. Placental size and shape are not associated with birth size (e.g. weight, birth weight ratio, length, ponderal index).

2B. Associations between maternal factors and birth size are mediated by variation in placental size and shape.

Specific Aim 3: To determine whether and how placental size and shape influence offspring growth. Offspring growth was measured at multiple time points beginning at age 1 year up to 15 years of age and includes measures of adiposity (DXA assessed fat mass and fat distribution), vascular function (blood pressure, pulse pressure and endothelial function), and metabolic function (fasting glucose, insulin and lipids). The following null hypotheses will be tested:

3A. Placental size and shape are not associated with offspring childhood adiposity.

3B. Placental size and shape are not associated with offspring childhood vascular function.

3C. Placental size and shape are not associated with offspring childhood metabolic function.

3D. Associations between placental size and shape with offspring outcomes are mediated by effects of the placenta on birth size.

Specific Aim 4: To investigate the role of genetic variation on placental size and shape. DNA was extracted from mothers and offspring in the parent study. The following null hypotheses will be tested:

4A. Genetic variation does not explain variation in placental size and shape.

4B. Interactions between genes and the selected maternal factors (BMI, weight gain, diet) do not explain additional variations in placental size or shape.

Specific Aim 5: To develop and validate a placental index to identify offspring at risk for adverse outcomes with regard to adiposity, vascular function, and metabolic function.

Pilot analyses for the program subprojects will be generated using the placental data already collected from the Children in Focus subset (CIF, N=1050), and intermediate neurocognitive outcomes (TBD by Prof Golding), intermediate pulmonary function outcomes (TBD by Dr Henderson), and BMI, waist & fat & lean mass (DXA) at age 9 as previously determined by Dr Lawlor. These pilot analyses will provide strong support for our (generally well received) October submission, and we are optimistic about receiving funding based on our July resubmission. Dawn Misra and Jeremy Miles will work together on data analysis for the 3 subprojects, with Colin Steer additionally on the neurocognitive subproject.

Annex

1. Temperament and behaviour of child: Infant temperament using the Carey

at ages 6 and 24 months; EAS temperament scale at 42 months;

Behaviour measures of hyperactivity, antisocial/conduct problems, peer

problems, emotional behaviour and prosocial behaviour as well as overall

behaviour difficulties, measured at ages 4 - 13 years

2. Child development: mental and motor development measured annually

between 6 months and 4 years

3. Cognitive measures: IQ using the WISC at age 8; short term memory;

executive function;

4. Speech and language: Vocabulary measured from 15 months to 3 years;

Grammatical understanding; Phonological problems; Use of speech

tharapist

5. Motor coordination: Balance tests; fine motor coordination; Gross motor

coordination; Functional motor impairment, including DCD

6. Childhood growth: Height; Weight; Head circumference; Hip

circumference; Waist circumference; Body mass index - all at various time

points from 4 months to 15 years

7. Educational attainments: Results of SATS tests at 7, 11 and 14; GCSE

results; Results of ALSPAC's own tests ofReading, Spelling, Mathematics,

and Science understanding.

8. Attention and hyperactivity: Hyperactive and inattentive behaviour using

questions to parents and teachers from 4 to 11 years and to the children

at 13.

9. Hearing ability: Tests of hearing under standardised conditions at ages 7,

9 and 11 - using audiometry and otoacoustic emissions; Hearing of

speech discrimination at 3.5 and 5 years

10. Visual ability: Tests of visual acuity, stereoacuity, and other features of

visual ability at various ages from 8 months to 11 years

11. Other sensory outcomes (taste and smell): Taste test (PROP) results at

age 10; Smell test results at age 11

12. Childhood psychiatric disorders: Autism; ADHD; ADD; Anorexia nervosa;

Bulimia; Depression; Anxiety disorders; Conduct disorder;

Date proposal received: 
Sunday, 20 September, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Sunday, 20 September, 2009
Keywords: 
Placenta
Primary keyword: 

B877 - Do associations between adolescent mental health and self-harm differ in males and females - 18/09/2009

B number: 
B877
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Emily Klineberg (University of Bristol, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Prof David Gunnell (University of Bristol, UK), Dr Judi Kidger (University of Bristol, UK)
Title of project: 
Do associations between adolescent mental health and self-harm differ in males and females?
Proposal summary: 

Background:

Community-based research indicates that over 10% of adolescents aged 15-16 have self-harmed at some point their lives (1). As only a small proportion of young people who self-harm seek formal help, further research is required at a population level to explore potentially modifiable factors associated with this behaviour (1,2). There has been limited prospective research predicting adolescent self-harm in community samples, particularly in the UK (3-7). Functions of self-harm vary between people, and over time; including being a short-term release of passing distress, or an aspect of an enduring mental illness.

Self-harm is a strong predictor of later suicide (8-9) and further self-harm (10-11). The prevalence of self-harm varies by gender in adolescents, with substantially higher rates in girls (1-6). For example, a cross-sectional school-based survey in Oxfordshire reported that 11.2% of females and 3.2% of males aged 15-16 years had self-harmed in the previous year (1). This contrasts with the incidence of suicide being 3-4 times higher in males complared with females. The relationship between self-injurious behaviour and gender is not entirely clear. Self-harm may serve a different function for young males and females.

Analysis included in EK's recently submitted PhD examined data from longitudinal school-based study examining risk and protective factors for self-harm in a sample of 1023 East London adolescents (Research with East London Adolescents; Community Health Survey; www.relachs.org). Cross-sectional associations between self-harm and sub-clinical psychological distress, such as borderline scores on the SDQ, and longitudinal associations between self-harm, conduct problems (SDQ) and depressive symptoms (assessed with the Short Moods and Feelings Questionnaire), illustrated that self-harm may relate to different aspects of mental health, as well as different levels of symptom severity.

There was some evidence for a gender difference in the relationships between mental health and self-harm, with an association between previous depressive symptoms and self-harm in boys (OR 6.40, 95%CI 2.14-20.60, p=0.01), which was not evident in girls. The sample size limited the analysis and there was insufficient power to explore this further, thus research with a larger sample is required. Gaining more insight into whether adolescent self-harm has different implications in young males and females has potential to inform targeted interventions and responses to self-harm. Associations between self-harm and sub-threshold psychological distress may imply that interventions need to focus on general emotional health and well-being, not necessarily the recognition of and response to diagnosable mental health problems. The proposed analysis would examine gender differences in relationships between mental health in early adolescence (11-13 years) and self-harm at age 16.

Proposed analysis:

This proposal will constitute part of an application (by EK, mentored by DG and JK) for a 1 year ESRC postdoctoral fellowship, in which a limited amount of additional research related to PhD may be undertaken in addition to writing papers for publication from doctoral research.

The proposed analysis will aim to examine data already collected as part of ALSPAC, addressing two main research questions:

(i) Which aspects of mental health measured at ages 11 and 13 have prospective associations with self-harm at age 16?

(ii) Are there gender differences in the associations between mental health and self-harm?

Multivariable logistic regression would be used to examine associations with self-harm reported at age 16+, by gender. Potential predictors would include self-harm at age 11, assessed as a part of Borderline Personality Disorder, and subscales from psychological measures (DAWBA and SDQ) assessing depressive symptoms, emotional problems, conduct problems and oppositional behaviour at ages 11 and 13. Additionally, demographic data, and personality factors such as sensation seeking, locus of control and self-concept may be relevant to consider.

References:

1. Hawton, K. et al (2002) 'Deliberate self harm in adolescents: self report survey in schools in England', British Medical Journal, 325; 1207-11.

2. Fortune, S. et al (2008) 'Help seeking before and after episodes of self-harm: a descriptive study in school pupils in England', BMC Public Health, 8:369.

3. Evans, E. et al (2004) 'Factors associated with suicidal phenomena in adolescents: A systematic review of population-based studies', Clinical Psychology Review, 24:957-79.

4. Patton, G. et al (1997) 'Adolescent suicidal behaviours: a population-based study of risk', Psychological Medicine, 27: 715-24.

5. Young, R. et al (2006) 'Prevalence of deliberate self-harm and attempted suicide within contemporary Goth youth subculture: longitudinal cohort study', British Medical Journal, 332: 1058-61.

6. Martin, G. et al (2005) 'Perceived academic performance, self-esteem and locus of control as indicators of need for assessment of adolescent suicide risk: implications for teachers', Journal of Adolescence, 28:75-87.

7. Haavisto, A. et al (2005) 'Factors associated with ideation and acts of deliberate self-harm among 18 year old boys', Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 40:912-21.

8. Hawton, K. et al (1999) 'Suicide in young people: a study of 174 cases, aged under 25 years, based on coroners' and medical records', British Journal of Psychiatry, 175:1-6.

9. Cooper, J. et al (2005) 'Suicide After Deliberate Self-Harm: A 4-Year Cohort Study', American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(2):297-303.

10. Hawton, K. et al (2000) 'Deliberate self-harm in adolescents in Oxford, 1985-1995', Journal of Adolescence, 23: 47-55.

11. Hawton, K. et al (2003) 'Deliberate self-harm in adolescents: a study of characteristics and trends in Oxford, 1990-2000', Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(8): 1191-1198.

Date proposal received: 
Friday, 18 September, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Friday, 18 September, 2009
Keywords: 
Mental Health, Self-harm
Primary keyword: 

B874 - Association of CHRNA3/A5 and COMT Genotypes with Smoking Cessation - 16/09/2009

B number: 
B874
Principal applicant name: 
Prof Marcus Munafo (University of Bristol, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Prof George Davey Smith (University of Bristol, UK), Dr Rachel Freathy (Peninsula Medical School, University of Plymouth, UK), Dr Sarah J Lewis (University of Bristol, UK), Dr Nic Timpson (University of Bristol, UK)
Title of project: 
Association of CHRNA3/A5 and COMT Genotypes with Smoking Cessation.
Proposal summary: 

We propose to test the hypothesis that genetic variation at two loci will reduce the likelihood of stopping smoking during the perinatal period. We expect these effects to be independent and will not investigate them in combination.

The first SNP (rs1051730) is located within the CHRNA3/A5 gene cluster, which encodes the nicotinic receptor alpha-3 and alpha-5 sub-units. Itwas originally reported to be associated with nicotine dependence in 2008 (Thorgeirsson et al., 2008), and this association has subsequently replicated in most (but not all) studies published since. It has also been investigated in relation to smoking cessation (e.g., Freathy et al., 2009). The National Institute of Drug Abuse (NIDA) has formed a working group to investigate the strength of evidence for the association of SNPs in chromosome 15 (including rs1051730) with smoking cessation. We propose to share the ALSPAC data published by Freathy and colleagues with this collaborative initiative, given the importance of including large, high-quality data sets of this kind.

The second SNP (rs4680) is located within the COMT gene, which encodes the catechol-O-methyltransferase enzyme, responsible for the degradation of extracellular dopamine, in particular in the prefrontal cortex. It has been widely investigated across a range of psychiatric phenotypes. We have recently reported an association between this SNP and smoking cessation in two clinical trials (Munafo et al., 2008; Johnstone et al., 2007), but subsequent attempts at replication have been inconsistent. One study found evidence of association in two independent samples of females only (Collila et al., 2005). Given evidence that oestrogen modulates COMT gene transcription (e.g., Jiang et al., 2003), it is possible that any effects of COMT genotype may be stronger in females.

Additional data on maternal depression over the same period are requested in order to explore whether any genetic associations with smoking cessation also predict change in depression symptom score, given the uncertainty regarding the direction of causation between cigarette smoking and depression.

Specific hypotheses:

1. The rs1051730 T allele will be associated with a reduced likelihood of smoking cessation;

2. The rs4680 G allele will be associated with a reduced likelihood of smoking cessation.

ALSPAC phenotypes required:

1. Maternal smoking status before, during and after pregnancy;

2. Maternal daily cigarette consumption and years smoked;

3. Maternal depression symptoms (EPDS) before, during and after pregnancy;

4. Maternal ancestry, age, educational attainment.

ALSPAC genotypes required:

1. rs1051730;

2. rs4680.

Date proposal received: 
Wednesday, 16 September, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Wednesday, 16 September, 2009
Keywords: 
Genetics, Smoking
Primary keyword: 

B873 - Investigation of the effect of maternal iodine status on cognitive and behavioural outcomes in the offspring - 08/09/2009

B number: 
B873
Principal applicant name: 
Prof Margaret Rayman (University of Surrey, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Dr Sarah Bath (University of Surrey, UK), Dr Pauline Emmett (University of Bristol, UK), Prof Jean Golding (University of Bristol, UK), Mr Colin Steer (University of Bristol, UK)
Title of project: 
Investigation of the effect of maternal iodine status on cognitive and behavioural outcomes in the offspring.
Proposal summary: 

We note that there has already been an ALSPAC study showing an effect of fish intake during pregnancy on I.Q in the child. Fish is a rich source of iodine which may, at the very least, have contributed to the beneficial effects of fish observed. Therefore, this project would build on this association and evaluate the link between maternal iodine status during pregnancy and developmental scores in the offspring.

Background

Iodine is essential for the production of thyroid hormones, thyroxine (T4) and triiodityronine (T3). The mother is an important source of thyroid hormones to the developing fetus, particularly in the first trimester before the onset of fetal thyroid function. Thyroid hormones are required for the neurological development of the child.

The recommended intake of iodine in the UK is 140 mcg/day and no increment is recommended during pregnancy. However, more recently the World Health Organisation has advised 250mcg of iodine per day during pregnancy.

Iodine deficiency during pregnancy in the most severe form, leads to development of cretinism in the offspring. However, studies have also demonstrated that mild to moderate maternal iodine deficiency, and the subsequent effect on thyroid hormone concentration, can lead to a spectrum of disorders in the offspring including reduction in psychomotor development, reduction in IQ, and ADHD development.

The recommended method of assessing the adequacy of iodine intake in a population, is through urinary iodine concentration measurements. This is because over 90% of ingested iodine is ultimately excreted in the urine and therefore is a useful biological marker of recent iodine intake.

Historically iodine deficiency was common in several parts of the U.K, and a goitre belt was documented from the West Country up to the Cotswalds and Derbyshire, including the area of Avon. Though the UK is currently considered to be iodine sufficient, the status of iodine in the population is not monitored through urine measurements. Data on iodine from the National Diet and Nutrition Survey is based on food diary analysis, which is not as accurate as urinary measures. However, this indicates that young women (19-24 years) have mean intakes below the RNI and that 12% of this age group of women had intakes less than the lower reference nutrient intake. In addition to the NDNS data, other studies in the UK have measured urinary iodine in pregnant women, which revealed mild to moderate deficiency in some women. We have assessed iodine deficiency in a group of women of childbearing age from the University of Surrey: 33% of the women had an iodine intake below the reference nutrient intake (RNI) and had they become pregnant, 79% of them would have been below the current WHO requirements for pregnancy.

Study proposal:

To date, there are no studies in the UK which assess the impact of mild to moderate maternal iodine deficiency on child (or birth) outcome. It is therefore proposed to use the ALSPAC urine samples to measure iodine status in pregnancy and to link that to the existing ALSPAC data on pregnancy and birth outcomes, IQ and other developmental scores in the offspring. Through the use of the food frequency questionnaires, foods that contribute to iodine status can be evaluated against outcome measures in order to provide practical dietary advice for women during pregnancy.

In order to asses iodine in urine, approximately 1.2 ml of urine would be required from the frozen stored samples. We plan to analyse some 1000 urine samples and the outcomes listed above will be evaluated in the children of the pregnant women studied. (Sarah Bath, my PhD student, could assist in pulling th4 samples from the freezer if that would help.) The food frequency questionnaires would be assessed with particular attention to foods rich in iodine (fish, milk and dairy products) to determine the foods that contribute the most to the iodine status in this group of women.

Selection of urine samples will use that gestation (which should be as near to 12 weeks as possible) for individuals for whom most outcomes are available (especially for IQ at age 8, and where possible WPSSI at age 4).

The urine samples will be analysed in Southampton. Iodine determination will be carried out by ICP-MS.

The statistical analyses will be undertaken by the PhD student, Sarah Bath, under the guidance of Colin Steer.

Date proposal received: 
Tuesday, 8 September, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Tuesday, 8 September, 2009
Keywords: 
Cognitive Function, Environment
Primary keyword: 

B872 - Exposure to alcohol during pregnancy childhood development and teenage drinking a study of trans-generational effects - 06/09/2009

B number: 
B872
Principal applicant name: 
Ms Luisa Zuccolo (University of Bristol, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Prof George Davey Smith (University of Bristol, UK)
Title of project: 
Exposure to alcohol during pregnancy, childhood development and teenage drinking ? a study of trans-generational effects.
Proposal summary: 

Aims

The main aim of the proposed study is to investigate the role of prenatal alcohol exposure on offspring cognition, neurological and motor development. I will also study maternal drinking patterns and how they influence offspring's alcohol behaviour including the onset of problem drinking, both directly and indirectly through a chain of biological effects on development and their consequences on educational attainment and choice of peer groups. The focus will be on causal effects and intergenerational determinants of health and health-related behaviours.

Date proposal received: 
Sunday, 6 September, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Sunday, 6 September, 2009
Keywords: 
Alcohol, Development, Epigenetics , Pregnancy
Primary keyword: 

B869 - RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN RANKL POLYMORPHISMS AND SERUM PROTEIN LEVELS - 02/09/2009

B number: 
B869
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Jon Tobias (University of Bristol, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Dr Dave Evans (University of Bristol, UK), Prof Jeff Holly (University of Bristol, UK), Mr John P Kemp (University of Bristol, UK)
Title of project: 
RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN RANKL POLYMORPHISMS AND SERUM PROTEIN LEVELS.
Proposal summary: 

Background

A highly co-ordinated and regulated process known as bone remodelling maintains the structural integrity and mineral density of bone. During bone remodelling, worn or damaged surfaces of trabecular and cortical bone are resorbed by osteoclast cells and replaced by osteoblast cells 1,2. The extent to which bone resorption (osteolysis) and synthesis (osteogenesis) occur are dependent on the interaction of the following three principle cytokines: tumour necrosis factor ligand superfamily member receptor activator of nuclear factor-cob (NF-kB) ligand (RANKL); its cellular receptor, receptor activator of NF-kB (RANK), and the decoy receptor, osteoprotegerin (OPG) 3.

Osteolysis is initiated by RANKL/RANK signalling. RANKL is secreted by osteoblastic cells and specifically recognised and bound by RANK receptors of osteoclastic cells. RANKL binding catalyses the formation, activation and maintenance of multinucleated osteoclasts (osteoclastogenesis), which attach to and resorb bone 3. Bone resorption is in turn regulated by a third cytokine called OPG. OPG is a soluble decoy receptor that binds to and sequesters RANKL, thereby limiting the activation of RANK and reducing osteoclastogenesis and osteolysis3 4. As a result the ratio of RANKL/OPG is an important determinant of bone mineral density (BMD) and skeletal integrity. Inherent perturbations of this ratio contribute to a variety of pathological conditions ranging from osteoporosis (low BMD) to osteopetrosis (high BMD) 4. Recently allelic variants of Rankl and Opg have been correlated with varying gene expression profiles and bone mineral density in patients with osteoporosis 5 6. These observations highlight the potential effects allelic variation may have on RANKL/OPG ratios and BMD.

Our research group has recently completed a genome wide association study based on tibial pQCT scans obtained in ALSPAC children at age 15. We identified several allelic variants which are located within and adjacent to Rankl which show associations with cortical BMD at genome-wide significance levels We hypothesise that these allelic variants may influence the steady state levels of RANKL sufficiently to alter the bone remodelling process thereby influencing BMD.

Aims

1. To quantify the steady state levels of serum RANKL in a cohort of 200 ALSPAC subjects who are homozygous and heterozygous for a specific Rankl marker.

2. To determine if genotypic variation at a single Rankl marker accounts for variation in RANKL steady state level and BMD.

Methods

A small sub-group of children (~200 individuals) from ALSPAC will be selected on the basis of being homozygous for the common and rare Rankl allele which is most strongly associated with cortical BMD (as identified in our recent genome-wide association scan). It is proposed that these individuals will have their RANKL protein levels quantified using the ampli-sRANKL enzyme immuno assay kit (Biomedica, Geneva) at Professor J Holly's laboratory at Southmead Hospital. Statistical association between RANKL protein levels and Rankl genotypes will be tested. Association will also be tested between RANKL protein levels and bone mineral density (as assessed by DXA and pQCT measurements).

Literature cited

1. Teitelbaum, S.L. & Ross, F.P. Genetic regulation of osteoclast development and function. Nat. Rev. Genet 4, 638-649(2003).

2. Martin, T.J. & Sims, N.A. Osteoclast-derived activity in the coupling of bone formation to resorption. Trends in Molecular Medicine 11, 76-81(2005).

3. Boyce, B.F. & Xing, L. Functions of RANKL/RANK/OPG in bone modeling and remodeling. Arch Biochem Biophys. 473, 139-146(2008).

4. Simonet, W. et al. Osteoprotegerin: A Novel Secreted Protein Involved in the Regulation of Bone Density. Cell 89, 309-319(1997).

5. Hsu, Y. et al. Variation in genes involved in the RANKL/RANK/OPG bone remodelling pathway are associated with bone mineral density at different skeletal sites in men. Human Genetics 118, 568-577(2006).

6. Takacs, I. et al. Allelic variations of RANK/RANKL/OPG signaling system is related to bone mineral density and in vivo gene expression. Bone 44, S116(2009).

Date proposal received: 
Wednesday, 2 September, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Wednesday, 2 September, 2009
Keywords: 
Genetics
Primary keyword: 

B871 - MATERNAL DEPRESSION AND SOCIAL SUPPORT - 27/08/2009

B number: 
B871
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Fariza Yahya (University of Bristol, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Prof Ricardo Araya (University of Bristol, UK), Dr Jonathan Evans (University of Bristol, UK)
Title of project: 
MATERNAL DEPRESSION AND SOCIAL SUPPORT.
Proposal summary: 

BACKGROUND

Depression is much commoner in women than men (Weissman, 1995). The high prevalence of depression in women is most likely due to a combination of gender-related differences in cognitive styles, certain biologic factors and a higher incidence of psychosocial and economic stresses in women (Korstein,1997). Depression during the perinatal period is common and it is debated if it is of a different type. Perinatal depression is often divided into antenatal and postnatal depression.

Postnatal Depression: Depression up to one year after delivery is called postpartum depression or postnatal depression. Postnatal depression is a mental and behavioural disorder associated with the puerperium, typically commencing within 6 weeks of delivery according to the International Classification of Disease-ICD-10(World Health Organisation, 1992). It is characterised by episodes of irritability, guilt, exhaustion, anxiety, sleep disorders and somatic symptoms which have a disabling effect on mothers, children and families of sufferers, following childbirth (Green et al., 1990). The cause of postpartum depression remains unclear with diverse researchers suggesting a multi-factorial aetiology (Ross et al., 2004). However, epidemiological studies strongly support the importance of psychosocial variables (O'Hara & Swain 1996, Beck 2001). In particular, a recent meta-analysis of 84 studies showed several risk factors including low levels of social support, stressful life events, childcare stress, low self esteem, low income, and marital dissatisfaction (Beck 2001). Buuljen (2007) concluded that one of the most notable explanations for PND was the lack of social support.

Social support: Social support is defined as help in difficult life situations. Social support is a concept that is generally understood in an intuitive sense, as the help from other people in a difficult life situations. There are different types and sources of social support. House described four main categories of social support: emotional, appraisal, informational and instrumental (House, 1981). Based on Barrera's (1981,2000) theoretical model, social support in the perinatal period can be defined as activities directed at assisting pregnant women and new mothers in mastering emotional distress, sharing tasks, giving advice, and teaching parenting skills, including the provision of material aid.

Social support seems to be one of the most important protective factors against depression in women at all periods of their lives; lack of social support has been consistently associated with depression in women of childbearing age. In order to understand better the relationship between social support and postnatal depression in women, I did a systematic review.

Social support and postnatal depression: In my sytematic review I was able to identify 20 papers that used a longitudinal design to assess the association between social support in pregnancy and postnatal depression. Among those that measured social support during the antenatal period, Brugha et al (1998), Glasser et al (2000), Bernazzani at al (1997) and Webster at al (2000) measured social support at less than 28 weeks of gestation; whereas Rahman et al (2003) and Chee et al (2008) took their measurements at the third trimester, between thirty four to forty weeks of gestations. Brugha et al (1998), Glasser et al (2000) and Webster et al (2000) found strong association between low social support at antenatal period and high EPDS score at postnatal period. Rahman et al (2003) found that support related to childcare by at least one of family member was associated with less PND, and the finding was almost similar to the study done by Chee et al (2000) when she found low instrumental support was associated with less PND but not for emotional support. Therefore from the studies we noticed that the measurement of social support was quite general when it was measured at early gestations, but when it was measured at late gestations the authors started to focus on type of social support.

Therefore my proposed study will be base on the objectives below.

OBJECTIVES

1. To study the association between social support in pregnancy and postnatal depression

2. To investigate the association between different types of social support in pregnancy and postnatal depression

3. To study if other psychological and social factors in pregnancy may contribute to postnatal depression in the presence of good social support.

METHODOLOGY

The proposed project will benefit from the rich and extensive longitudinal data obtained by Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC). ALSPAC is an ongoing population based study. This study will investigate the importance of social support in the ALSPAC mothers during pregnancy and the postnatal period.

Measures:

1. Depressive symptoms (mothers); antenatal and postnatal

2. Social support and social network measures

3. Personality

4. Religiosity

5. Locus of control

6. Life events

Analysis plan:

We will use complete datasets for the variables of interest. Initially I will perform a descriptive analysis of the main variables of interest during pregnancy and one year of postnatal period. I will then perform some basic cross-tabulations between social support measures during pregnancy and postnatal depression. Chi-square tests for these associations will be estimated. I will then examine the possible influence of confounding variables such as personality and life events. After this descriptive phase I will perform multivariate regression analysis to examine the association between postnatal depression and social support in pregnancy before and after adjusting for possible confounders. Potential moderating and mediating factors will also be examined. Finally I will explore applying other statistical approaches using more sophisticated models such as structural equation modelling.

Date proposal received: 
Thursday, 27 August, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Thursday, 27 August, 2009
Keywords: 
Depression, Social Science
Primary keyword: 

B867 - Lead and child health development - 24/08/2009

B number: 
B867
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Caroline M Taylor (University of Bristol, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Prof Alan Emond (University of Bristol, UK), Prof Jean Golding (University of Bristol, UK)
Title of project: 
Lead and child health development.
Proposal summary: 

Although in the past there was concern over the dangers to children of blood lead levels over 25 microg/dL, there is increasing evidence that much lower levels (as low as 5microg/dL have a deleterious effect on child development (see an ALSPAC paper about to be published in Archives of Disease in Childhood).

Further studies are needed to answer the following questions: (a) when in development does the exposure have the most harmful effects? (b) where does the majority of the lead come from? (c) which biological samples are most useful in epidemiological studies of child development?

The programme will use various samples from the ALSPAC survey and develop appropriate measurement methodologies for testing the different sample types. It will compare lead exposure measured in children's teeth over time, distinguishing between levels laid down prenatally, in infancy and later childhood, with maternal whole blood prenatal levels, infant hair and toenails, childhood levels from age 2.5 years, and 7 years. Lead isotope levels will also be measured to assess the likely exposure sources. The aim will be to develop appropriate methods for studies of other population surveys and, when appropriate, for screening the population for appropriate intervention.

In parallel, the epidemiological data concerning maternal prenatal levels of lead, 5000 of which are currently being measured at CDC in Atlanta, will be analysed statistically to determine (i) how closely the prenatal maternal levels correlate with the 2.5 year old measures; (ii) whether the maternal prenatal lead levels are associated with outcomes of pregnancy such as preterm delivery; (iii) whether the relationships with adverse neurocognitive outcomes are predicted with more efficiency using prenatal assays than later measures of child blood.[if so this would be an important time to screen for lead levels].

Date proposal received: 
Monday, 24 August, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Monday, 24 August, 2009
Keywords: 
Development
Primary keyword: 

B868 - Tracking Accelerometry Derived Sedentary Behaviour and Obesity from Childhood to Adolescence - 19/08/2009

B number: 
B868
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Russ Pate (University of South Carolina, Columbia)
Co-applicants: 
Dr Steven Blair (University of South Carolina, Columbia), Dr Marsha Dowda (University of South Carolina, Columbia), Prof Jonathan Mitchell (University of South Carolina, Columbia), Prof Chris Riddoch (University of Bath, UK), Mr Calum Mattocks (University of Bristol, UK), Prof Andy Ness (University of Bristol, UK)
Title of project: 
Tracking Accelerometry Derived Sedentary Behaviour and Obesity from Childhood to Adolescence.
Proposal summary: 

Purpose:

To determine the extent that sedentary behaviour and measures of obesity track from childhood to adolescence.

Background:

Tracking of sedentary behaviour from childhood to adolescence has not been extensively studied, and few studies have tracked accelerometry derived sedentary behaviour (1). Using objectively acquired sedentary time at repeated time points is important since the reliability of self-reported sedentary behaviour is likely to vary with age. The study by Baggett et al. (1) found fair-to-moderate tracking of sedentary behaviour, from 12 to 14-years, in sample of girls (kappa=0.17). A second study regarding accelerometer derived sedentary time found that, from 6 to 9-years, there was moderate tracking (r=0.37-0.52) (2).

The use of DXA derived fat mass as a measure of obesity is highly advantageous over certain anthropometric measures of obesity, as adolescence is a period when lean mass is increasing in addition to fat mass (especially among males). The findings by Cheng et al. (3) found that the tracking of fat mass, from 11 to 18-years, was good (r=0.65); however, only Finnish girls (n=396) were included in the analysis.

Dependent Variable:

- Sedentary Behaviour (Tertiles)

- Obesity [Percentiles: 90th (obese), 80th (overweight) & 10th - 79th (normal)]

Indpendent Variables:

- Age (time)

Covariates:

- Maturation (Tanner)

- Parental occupation (SES)

- MVPA

Data Analysis:

- General Estimating Equations (GEE)

- Analyses will be conducted by gender (due to the different stages of maturation) and by SES (to determine the extent of tracking according to SES)

Target Journals:

MSSE

British Journal of Sports Medicine.

Date proposal received: 
Wednesday, 19 August, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Wednesday, 19 August, 2009
Keywords: 
Obesity
Primary keyword: 

B865 - Developing algorithms for assessment referral and management of childhood obesity in primary care - 14/08/2009

B number: 
B865
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Sanjay Kinra (London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Dr Ulla Sovio (London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, UK)
Title of project: 
Developing algorithms for assessment, referral and management of childhood obesity in primary care.
Proposal summary: 

Background:

NICE has called for greater support for frontline staff for the management of childhood obesity, who are often limited in dealing with these patients by lack of time and specialist training. However, whilst guidance statements and generic advice are available, few practical management tools for untrained staff currently exist. A detailed clinical tool based on a series of validated questions has been shown to be feasible in the secondary care setting in New Zealand. A similar tool for the management of adult obesity has been piloted in one PCT in England. The use of an electronic tool in the management of childhood obesity could assist practice nurses in the management and appropriate referral of childhood obesity, overcoming the issues of lack of time and training.

Aim:

To develop algorithms for use in the assessment, referral and management of childhood obesity in primary care.

The algorithms will: 1) help to identify those young people at greatest risk of obesity co-morbidities; and 2) identify characteristics of patients that will aid management decisions, e.g. child's current diet, level of physical activity, or psychological state.

The algorithms developed in this project will be incorporated into an electronic tool to assist practice nurses in the referral and management of obese children.

Methods:

- Potential data items for analysis will be identified through expert interviews and systematic reviews of the literature on childhood obesity management, co-morbidities, co-morbidity risk factors, and existing clinical algorithms for risk assessment and referral.

- Based on the preliminary list established through systematic reviews, data items from ALSPAC will be selected and analysed to identify variables that best predict obesity co-morbidities in this population. Data will also be analysed to identify characteristics of patients that will aid practice nurses in management decisions (including readiness to change, diet, activity, sedentary behaviours, sociodemographic characteristics).

- From these lists, eligible data items will be short-listed through a pre-defined selection process, which will include assessment of face validity, relevance, and sensitivity/specificity/predictive value in the UK population and feasibility of data collection in the primary care setting, to produce a list of approximately 20 items. Algorithms based on these items will be generated and incorporated into an electronic tool that identifies children at risk of co-morbidities, and generates personalised management plans.

Potential data items for the tool:

Assessment of co-morbidities

- Sex

- Age

- Ethnicity

- BMI

- Family history of disease/treatment/hospitalization/medical diagnoses

- Premature puberty

- Learning difficulties, syndromic obesity

- Skin - acanthosis nigricans

- Blood pressure

- Body fat distribution - wiast circumference, thigh circumference etc

Childhood obesity co-morbidities

- Nonalcoholic fatty liver disease (ALT)

- Impaired thyroid function (TF3, TF4)

- Components of metabolic syndrome:

- Hyperinsulinemia/insulin resistance

- Dyslipidemia

- Hypertension

- Proteinuria

- Prediabetes/type 2 diabetes

- Gastroesophageal reflux

- Musculoskeletal disorders, e.g. slipped capital femoral epiphysis and Blount disease

- Gallstones

- Asthma

- Polycystic ovarian syndrome

- Obstructive sleep apnoea

- Mood disorders/depression

Factors affecting treatment decisions

- Diet: level of consumption, consumption of fruit/veg, fizzy drinks, snacks between meals, portion size

- Activity: hours of organised activity, leisure activity, walking

- Sedentary behaviours: screen time, eating in front of TV

- Mood/depression

- Willingness to change

- Self-efficacy/confidence

- Family/environmental circumstances: free school meals, access to play area, who cooks at home

- Previous interventions

- Learning difficulties

- Behavioural problems

- Age, sex, ethnicity

- Degree of overweight.

Date proposal received: 
Friday, 14 August, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Friday, 14 August, 2009
Keywords: 
Obesity
Primary keyword: 

B864 - Gene Environment Processes Underlying the Development of Conduct Problem Trajectories SUPERCEDED BY B0990 - 13/08/2009

B number: 
B864
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Edward D Barker (University of Alabama, USA)
Co-applicants: 
Prof Barbara Maughan (King's College London, UK), Dr Thomas S Price (King's College London, UK), Dr Beate St. Pourcain (University of Bristol, UK)
Title of project: 
Gene Environment Processes Underlying the Development of Conduct Problem Trajectories (SUPERCEDED BY B0990).
Proposal summary: 

1. SUMMARY

Conduct problems (CP) include behaviors such as bullying/intimidating, initiating physical violence towards others, assault (stealing with confrontation), theft, vandalism, and running away from home. An estimated 5-10% of youth engaging in CP follow an early onset and persistent (EOP) trajectory, and account for over 50% of crime within a given community (Moffitt, 2006). The cost to both victims and communities is considerable (Foster). Nevertheless, well designed longitudinal studies converge on the finding that the development of CP is a heterogeneous process: at least two other trajectories have been consistently identified (Barker & Maughan, 2009; Odgers et al., 2007; 2008; Raine et al., 2005), neither of which appear to be completely free of risk in childhood nor adjustment problems thereafter. Preliminary findings from epidemiology studies suggest that youth may encounter environmental risk at different developmental periods, and that these risks (or protective factors) may interact within an individual's genotype to confer the onset or offset of CP.

Using previously published CP trajectories from ALSPAC (Barker & Maughan, 2009) as an outcome, we propose to examine environmental risk exposures, from gestation to the teens, and potential gene-to-CP effects, and gene-environment interactions. With regard to candidate polymorphisms, we propose to examine certain single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) and variable number tandem repeats (VNTRs) that have been shown to associate to variation in stress response and/or CP. We also propose to examine a meta-analytic genome wide association study (GWAS), between ALSPAC and the Twins Early Development Study.

2. BACKGROUND

Well designed longitudinal studies converge on the finding that the development of CP is a heterogeneous process: beyond the EOP subtype, at least two other trajectories of CP have been consistently identified (Barker & Maughan, 2009; Odgers et al., 2007; 2008; Raine et a., 2005), a childhood limited (CL) and an adolescent-onset (AO) subtype. Taken together, the results of these studies suggest heterogeneity in development poses a serious challenge for clinicians, interventionists, and researchers alike. Understanding etiologic pathways underlying these distinct trajectories is a significant public health concern and efforts should be focused not only on prevention, but on ensuring that children who present CP have the best possible outcomes.

Gaps in the current knowledge on the etiologies of these CP pathways are at least three. First, to date, studies typically have examined sets of risk at specific (or aggregated) developmental periods, but rarely have these risks incorporated the prenatal period and early postnatal periods, crucial times in child development that may bear on the timing and impact of important risks on the development of biological systems, and hence an individual's susceptibility to subsequent environmental risk exposure(s). Second, no matter the severity and duration of risks experienced by children, evidence highlights individual differences in response, with some individuals apparently protected, or more vulnerable, to later adverse outcomes (Rutter, 2009). That is, there is consensus that risk factors are likely to be complex and involve interplay amongst genetic vulnerabilities and environmental risk exposure. The third limitation is that most studies to date have examined the EOP pathway, highlighting the need for further studies to examine etiologies underlying the CL and AO pathways. Indeed, differences in the development of CP amongst these trajectories suggests that the variation and timing of environmental risk, and presumably genetic vulnerabilities (or protective factors), as key features for refining current knowledge of these taxons.

We propose to test the extent to which variations in development of CP is related to the developmental interplay of genetic and environmental child-based risks. Specifically, we propose to conduct theoretically driven analysis where the research focus includes environmental risk exposure during pregnancy (18 wks and 32 weeks), early postnatal (birth to age 2; age 2 to age 4), school entry (age 7-8), adolescence (age 15-16), as well as candidate genes.

3. METHODS

3.1 Overview

Phase 1. Examine developmental continuity (and discontinuity) of environmental risk exposures during the following developmental periods: antenatal, early childhood, primary school and adolescence.

Phase 2. Examine both direct effects (polymorphism-to-CP trajectory) and interactions between polymorphisms and the environmental risks (i.e., examine if the relationship between a polymorphism and a trajectory is moderated by environmental risk exposure). SNPS and VNTRs prioritized via previous CP studies and studies on variation in stress response will be typed in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC, PI: G. Davey-Smith; Golding, Pembrey, & Jones, 2001). These data will be used to test hypotheses of direct gene, gene-environment interaction and haplotype-environment interactions where the outcome is the the odds of following the different CP trajectories. The availability of prospective information on environmental exposure in this sample will allow hypotheses to be tested about the impact of timing of environmental stress for these different CP trajectories.

Phase 3. GWAS meta-analysis. I have negotiated access to the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS, PI R. Plomin; Trouton, Spinath, & Plomin, 2002). Dr. Beate Glaser, of ALSPAC, has agreed to collaborate and wil estimate the GWAS on ALSPAC. Dr. Glaser will also combine GWAS estimates obtained from TEDS with those from ALSPAC.

3.2 Data collection.

We propose to genotype SNPs and VNTRs, as well as haplotypes of CRH-R1 (e.g., Lake et al., 2003) in ALSPAC.

3.3 Existing Data Required

Environmental risks are located in the previous section. The SNPs and VNTRs we propose to genotype are located in the next section.

3.4 Data Analysis.

Phase 1: We will estimate and examine the configuration of risk within each development period by way of Latent Class Model (McCutcheon, 1987). Continuity of environmental risks will be examined via latent transition models (Muthen & Muthen, 2001-2006).

Phase 2: Examination of gene-environment processes

Phase 3: GWAS meta-analysis

3.5 Work already completed

We have already published a paper on the trajectories and antenatal and early postnatal risks associated to the EOP vs CL pathways (Barker & Maughan, 2009). We will soon submit a follow-up to this paper (Barker, Bonamy & Maughan, in preparation) for approval to the ALSPAC exec. These two papers create the substantive background for this application.

References

Barker, E. D., & Maughan (2009). Differentiating early-onset persistent versus childhood-limited conduct problem youth. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 900-908.

Barker, E. D., Bonamy, B., & Maughan, B. (in preparation). Time-varying covariates of conduct problem trajectories.

Golding, J., Pembrey, M., & Jones, R. (2001). ALSPAC--the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. I. Study methodology. Pediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology, 15, 74-87.

Lake, S. L., Lyon, H., Tantisira, K., Silverman, K. E., Weiss, S. T., Laird, N.M., Schaid, D.J. (2003). Estimation and Tests of Haplotype-Environment Interaction when Linkage Phase Is Ambiguous. Human Heredity, 55, 56-65.

Moffitt, T. E. (2006). Life-course persistent versus adolescence-limited antisocial behavior. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental Psychopathology (2nd ed., Vol. 3: Risk, disorder, and adaptation, pp. 570-598). NY: Wiley.

Muthen, L. K., & Muthen, B. O. (1998-2006). Mplus. Statistical analyses with latent variables. User's guide (4.1 ed.). Los Angeles: Muthen & Muthen.

Odgers, C. L., Caspi, A., Broadbent, J. M., Dickson, N., Hancox, R., Harringthon, H., et al. (2007). Conduct problem subtypes in males predict differential adult health burden. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64, 476-484.

Odgers, C. L., Moffitt, T. E., Broadbent, J. M., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., et al. (2008). Female and male antisocial trajectories: From childhood origins to adult outcomes. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 673-716.

Raine, A., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Loeber, R., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., & Lynam, D. R. (2005). Neurocognitive impairments in boys on the life-course persistent antisocial path. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 38-49.

Robins L. N. (1966). Deviant children grown up. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.

Trouton, A., Spinath, F. M., & Plomin, R. (2002). Twins Early Development Study (TEDS): A multivariate, longitudinal genetic investigation of language, cognition and behavior problems in childhood. Twin Research, 5(5), 444-448.

Date proposal received: 
Thursday, 13 August, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Thursday, 13 August, 2009
Keywords: 
Conduct Disorder , Genes
Primary keyword: 

B860 - ALSPAC mercury project - 07/08/2009

B number: 
B860
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Phiippe Grandjean (University of Southern Denmark, Europe)
Co-applicants: 
Title of project: 
ALSPAC mercury project.
Proposal summary: 

Methylmercury appears to have irreversible, adverse effects on the nervous system that are much more

widespread and serious in children than in adults, but better dose-response information is needed. The

proposed project will analyze 1,000 to 1,500 stored umbilical cords for mercury to determine the impact

of prenatal mercury exposure from maternal fish intake during pregnancy on the nervous system

development, which has already been assessed in the cohort subjects during 16 years of follow-up. This

information will be useful for future recommendations and guidelines on optimal seafood diets during

pregnancy.

The cords will be freeze-dried before analysis to obtain the dry-weight-based total mercury concentration

as the best available indicator of prenatal methylmercury exposure. This methodology has been

standardized and validated using large sample materials from birth cohorts in the Faroe Islands (where

cord tissue, cord blood, and other samples were available and were analysed for mercury).

Samples will be selected among the approximately 7,500 available to represent those that alfready have

GWAS data or otherwise the most complete phenotype data.

Follow-up through age 16 years will be utilized in regard to neurodevelopment and associated markers of

heart function as indicators of neurotoxicity. Possible adverse impacts of prenatal methylmercury

exposure on highly relevant neurodevelopmental functions will be examined, as will the possible

interactions caused by beneficial nutrients from maternal fish diets and the significance of relevant

heterogeneities involved in methylmercury metabolism and/or brain development processes that may be

susceptible to methylmercury toxicity.

The following three hypotheses will be examined: 1) Methylmercury-associated deficits in sensitive

domains are present at low-level prenatal methylmercury exposures and remain detectable through to

adolescence. 2) Beneficial nutrients from maternal seafood intake affect the same outcomes to

counterbalance methylmercury-associated deficits. And 3) Heterogeneities for metabolic and

neruodevelopmental genes affect the degree of methylmercury neurotoxicity.

Data analysis will be carried out jointly and will also include similar data from the largest Faroese birth

cohort (N = 1,000).

Date proposal received: 
Friday, 7 August, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Friday, 7 August, 2009
Keywords: 
Mercury
Primary keyword: 

B859 - Association study of mathematics ability and disability with multiple previously identified loci - 04/08/2009

B number: 
B859
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Sophia Docherty (King's College London, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Dr Beate St. Pourcain (University of Bristol, UK)
Title of project: 
Association study of mathematics ability and disability with multiple previously identified loci.
Proposal summary: 

Our group has already completed a three-stage genomewide association study of mathematical ability in 10 year-olds drawn from the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS) (under review). 43 SNPs were nominated from the first two stages which involved assaying pooled DNA on Affymetrix DNA microarrays to compare individuals of high mathematical ability to those of low mathematical ability. In the third stage, these 43 SNPs were individually genotyped in a sample which spanned the entire spectrum of ability. 10 SNP associations remained significant. Though effect sizes were extremely small, when a composite SNP-score was created for subjects in the final sample by summing the number of 'mathemtics score increasing alleles', these 10 SNPs accounted for 2.9% of the phenotypic variation in the sample. Interestingly, when the remaining 33 SNPs were added to this score, they did not mask this effect on mathematical performance, but rather increased it. This suggests to us that many of these remaining 33 SNPs may show true influences over mathematical ability which when analysed alone, our sample was underpowered to detect.

As the true test of association is replication, we are currently seeking out a comparable sample with mathematics ability data. With access to 1) existing ALSPAC mathematics scores, and 2) existing genotype data - collected on Illumina microarrays - we would be able to simply and easily test for the association of the 43 implicated SNPs (or where necessary, their proxies on the Illumina platform) with mathematical ability in the ALSPAC sample. As quantitative genetic results indicate that some of the genetic action over mathematics is general across ages, whilst some is not, we propose to investigate the action of the 43 SNPs over mathematical ability at all ages for which there is data available (we have been informed that this is 4-5, 11 and 15 years). Further to this, we are especially interested in investigating the combined influence of 1) the top 10 SNPs and 2) all 43 SNPs over mathematical ability. As mentioned above, this may be achieved by simply summing the math-increasing alleles possesed by each subject, and investigating the relationship of this SNP-set score with mathematical ability. If linear models are used to investigate SNP associations, then a simple alternative might be to add all 10/43 SNPs into the model. Whilst we would greatly prefer to conduct these analyses ourselves - we do understand that there may be restrictions on the release of some or all of the data we have requested, and so would also be very happy to work closely with ALSPAC researchers on this project.

As much of the work our group conducts is funded by the Wellcome Trust, we follow the same open access publishing policy as ALSPAC. We will therefore ensure that any publications resulting from this collaboration will be freely available to all researchers via PubMed Central.

Date proposal received: 
Tuesday, 4 August, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Tuesday, 4 August, 2009
Keywords: 
Social Science
Primary keyword: 

B856 - Effect of maternal testosterone levels on offspring 2D4D ratio - 03/08/2009

B number: 
B856
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Dave Evans (University of Bristol, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Prof George Davey Smith (University of Bristol, UK)
Title of project: 
Effect of maternal testosterone levels on offspring 2D:4D ratio.
Proposal summary: 

The SNP rs314277 in the LIN28B gene has recently been associated with age at first menarche in ALSPAC and several other population cohorts. Using genome-wide association analysis, we have recently found an association between this SNP and ratio of second to fourth digit length (2D:4D ratio) in the ALSPAC children. 2D:4D ratio is meant to be a marker of testosterone exposure in utero. I am interested in seeing whether levels of testosterone in ALSPAC mothers are correlated with offspring 2D:4D and if so whether they explain this SNP association. I am therefore asking for access to levels of testosterone and sex hormone binding globulin (SHBG) as measured in the ALSPAC mothers.

Date proposal received: 
Monday, 3 August, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Monday, 3 August, 2009
Keywords: 
Hormones
Primary keyword: 

B855 - The affect of parenting on child development and health - 29/07/2009

B number: 
B855
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Raghu Lingam (University of Bristol, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Prof Alan Emond (University of Bristol, UK), Dr Toity Deave (University of Bristol, UK), Prof Sarah Stewart-Brown (University of Warwick, UK), Dr Andrea Waylen (University of Bristol, UK)
Title of project: 
The affect of parenting on child development and health.
Proposal summary: 

ALSPAC is an important resource in the study of child development. Researchers using the cohort have shown an association between factors such as antenatal diet, maternal and paternal anxiety and depression and later child development.(1-5) There is strong evidence that child development and school performance are also influenced by socioeconomic factors including: parental occupation, marital status, income and parental education. (6-10) Maternal smoking and social adversity, specifically during pregnancy, were independently associated with an increase in risk of child symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity in a large multi-centre study of environmental risk factors for ADHD.(11) However, the causal mechanism of how childhood adversity affects development is unclear.

Early parent-child interaction is the basis of the attachment relationship which in turn allows the child to develop a template for social and emotional relationships.(12) Although there has been work looking at the effect of parenting and childhood attachment difficulties on general health, behaviour and self regulation, potentially mediated through the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, the evidence base for the effect of parenting on other aspects of childhood development and later scholastic competence is less clear.(13, 14) The NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development, looking at 1000 US families, showed that early attachment predicted emotional development and language skills in younger children but not cognitive functioning; however the effect of attachment was dependent on socioeconomic background.(15, 16)

Dr Waylen and Professor Stewart-Brown have worked extensively with ALSPAC and have developed and validated a parenting measure which incorporates parental reports of attitude, behaviours and feelings towards the child. This measure of parenting has been shown to be independently associated with maternal reported child health.(17) In addition, ALSPAC has repeated measures of self-reported parenting activity scores between 6 months and 38 months. Professor Emond, Dr Deave and Dr Lingam have experience in the analysis of the modified Denver Developmental Screening Test (modified DDST) measured at 6, 18, 30 and 42 months along with measures of later development including motor coordination, speech and language skills, non verbal skills, social communication, and academic ability (reading and spelling).(2, 18)

Research questions

  1. What is the effect of parenting on child development measured using standardised tests, controlling for potential confounding and modifying factors?
  2. To what extent does parenting mediate the effect of known risk factors for child development such as socioeconomic status and parental education?

Methods

The primary exposure variable will be the parenting measure developed by Dr Waylen and Professor Stewart-Brown. A secondary exposure variable Parenting Activity will be developed using the ALSPAC parenting activity scores.

Outcome variables will be the modified Denver Developmental Screening Test (modified DDST) measured at 6, 18 and 30 months and later developmental measures including inattention and hyperactivity using the Development and Well-Being Assessment- DAWBA; difficulties in social cognition (The Social and Communication Disorders Checklist- SCDC; poor reading skills (using the Weschler Objective Reading Dimensions- WORD and NARA II); expressive language and comprehension using the WOLD, nonverbal impairment using the DANVA and Cognitive ability using the abbreviated WISK III measured between 7 and 9 years. (19-25)

A series of multivariable linear regression and logistic regression models (for continuous and binary outcomes respectively) will be created to look at the associations between parenting measures and child development accounting for potential confounding and mediating factors identified from the literature. Adjustment will be made for the bias introduced by missing data using multiple imputation by chained equation.(26)

Variable needed

Dr Lingam already has a dataset containing the developmental variables and some of the parenting activity scores needed for the analysis as part of his PhD looking at the causes and consequences of developmental coordination disorder. Use of the derived parenting measure has been granted by Dr Waylen and Professor Stewart Brown. Full details of all the variables in the analysis are attached in table 1.

Power calculation

The prevalence of suboptimal parenting (increased hostility, resentment and hitting/shouting) has been reported in between 10 and 30% of the ALSPAC cohort at different time points.(17) Taking 7000 to be the study population (approximate children that attended focus at 7) and our outcome variable to be the lowest 10% of a given developmental trait, the relative risk that it will possible to be 80% sure of detecting at the 5% level of significance is between 1.27 and 1.42.

References

1. Hibbeln JR, Davis JM, Steer C, Emmett P, Rogers I, Williams C, et al. Maternal seafood consumption in pregnancy and neurodevelopmental outcomes in childhood (ALSPAC study): an observational cohort study. Lancet. 2007 Feb 17;369(9561):578-85.

2. Deave T, Heron J, Evans J, Emond A. The impact of maternal depression in pregnancy on early child development. BJOG. 2008 Jul;115(8):1043-51.

3. Ramchandani P, Stein A, Evans J, O'Connor TG. Paternal depression in the postnatal period and child development: a prospective population study. Lancet. 2005 Jun 25-Jul 1;365(9478):2201-5.

4. Ramchandani PG, Stein A, O'Connor TG, Heron J, Murray L, Evans J. Depression in men in the postnatal period and later child psychopathology: a population cohort study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008 Apr;47(4):390-8.

5. Talge NM, Neal C, Glover V. Antenatal maternal stress and long-term effects on child neurodevelopment: how and why? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007 Mar-Apr;48(3-4):245-61.

6. Brooks-Gunn J, Duncan GJ. The effects of poverty on children. Future Child. 1997 Summer-Fall;7(2):55-71.

7. Duncan GJ, Brooks-Gunn J. Family poverty, welfare reform, and child development. Child Dev. 2000 Jan-Feb;71(1):188-96.

8. Votruba-Drzal E. Economic disparities in middle childhood development: does income matter? Dev Psychol. 2006 Nov;42(6):1154-67.

9. Prathanee B, Thinkhamrop B, Dechongkit S. Factors associated with specific language impairment and later language development during early life: a literature review. Clin Pediatr (Phila). 2007 Jan;46(1):22-9.

10. Stanton-Chapman TL, Chapman DA, Bainbridge NL, Scott KG. Identification of early risk factors for language impairment. Res Dev Disabil. 2002 Nov-Dec;23(6):390-405.

11. Rodriguez A, Olsen J, Kotimaa AJ, Kaakinen M, Moilanen I, Henriksen TB, et al. Is prenatal alcohol exposure related to inattention and hyperactivity symptoms in children? Disentangling the effects of social adversity. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009 Feb 27.

12. Bowlby J. Attachment and loss: Attachment Vol1 London: Hogarth Press; 1969.

13. Rees CA. Thinking about children's attachments. Arch Dis Child. 2005 Oct;90(10):1058-65.

14. Stewart-Brown SL, Fletcher L, Wadsworth ME. Parent-child relationships and health problems in adulthood in three UK national birth cohort studies. Eur J Public Health. 2005 Dec;15(6):640-6.

15. Belsky J, Fearon RM. Infant-mother attachment security, contextual risk, and early development: a moderational analysis. Dev Psychopathol. 2002 Spring;14(2):293-310.

16. Friedman SL, Boyle DE. Attachment in US children experiencing nonmaternal care in the early 1990s. Attach Hum Dev. 2008 Sep;10(3):225-61.

17. Waylen A, Stallard N, Stewart-Brown S. Parenting and health in mid-childhood: a longitudinal study. Eur J Public Health. 2008 Jun;18(3):300-5.

18. Lingam R, Hunt L, Golding J, Jongmans M, Emond A. Prevalence of developmental coordination disorder using the DSM-IV at 7 years of age: a UK population-based study. Pediatrics. 2009 Apr;123(4):e693-700.

19. Frankenburg WK, Dodds J, Archer P. The Denver II: a major revision and restandardization of the Denver Developmental Screening Test Pediatrics. 1992;89 (1):91-7.

20. Goodman R, Ford T, Richards H, Gatward R, Meltzer H. The Development and Well-Being Assessment: description and initial validation of an integrated assessment of child and adolescent psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2000 Jul;41(5):645-55.

21. Skuse DH, James RS, Bishop DV, Coppin B, Dalton P, Aamodt-Leeper G, et al. Evidence from Turner's syndrome of an imprinted X-linked locus affecting cognitive function. Nature. 1997 Jun 12;387(6634):705-8.

22. Rust J, Golombok S, Trickey G. WORD Wechsler Objective Reading Dimensions Manual. Sidcup, UK: The Psychological Corporation; 1993.

23. Rust J. WOLD Wechsler Objective Language Dimensions Manual. London, UK: The Psychological Corporation; 1996.

24. Nowicki S, Duke MP. Individual differences in the nonverbal communication of affect: the Diagnostic Analysis of NonVerbal Accuracy scale. Journal of Nonverbal Behaviour. 1994;18:9-35.

25. Weschsler D, Golombok S, Rust J. WISC-IIIUK Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children -Third Edition UK Manual. 3rd ed. Sidcup, UK: The Psychological Corporation; 1992.

26. Stuart EA, Azur M, Frangakis C, Leaf P. Multiple imputation with large data sets: a case study of the Children's Mental Health Initiative. Am J Epidemiol. 2009 May 1;169(9):1133-9.

Date proposal received: 
Wednesday, 29 July, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Wednesday, 29 July, 2009
Keywords: 
Development
Primary keyword: 

B852 - The relationship between early emotionality and 5-HTTLPR genotype - 28/07/2009

B number: 
B852
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Argyris Stringaris (King's College London, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Prof Barbara Maughan (King's College London, UK)
Title of project: 
The relationship between early emotionality and 5-HTTLPR genotype.
Proposal summary: 

We wish to test the hypothesis that alleles leading to lower transcription rates of the serotonin transporter gene are associated with significantly higher levels of emotionality in early life. Our objective is to rigorously test this hypothesis by exploiting the size and representativeness of the unique ALSPAC sample and by using a research design that includes stringent control measures.

Identifying the genetic and environmental determinants of variation in human traits is seen as one of the central components of what is described as translational research in the biomedical sciences. Following this research direction, we propose using a large birth cohort in order to probe the relationship between a polymorphism in the promoter region of the serotonin transporter gene and a well-established early risk factor for psychiatric problems.

A paradigmatic case of studying the genetic underpinnings of emotions and behaviour has been that of establishing the association between human emotionality and a functional polymorphism at the promoter region of the serotonin transporter gene, designated 5-HTTLPR. Emotionality, commonly referred to as neuroticism in the literature, is measured as a continuous personality trait and has been shown to be an important risk factor for depressive and anxiety disorders. The short allele (S) at the 5-HTTLPR leads to lower transcriptional activity and , thus, reduced serotonin re-uptake at the synapse compared to the long (L) allele. A high-profile publication in the mid '90s reported that possession of the short allele (s) of the 5-HTTLPR is significantly associated with higher scores on neuroticism measures compared to possession of the long allele. Indeed, a series of replications and extensions have lent further support to this important finding; although there have also been notable non-replications as well as ambiguous meta-analytic results. Given the long-known links between neuroticism and depression/anxiety, and also the central role ascribed to serotonin metabolism in depression/anxiety, this finding has boosted the hopes for arriving at a molecular explanation of mental disorder.

One of the most important questions arising in view of the potential importance of these findings is how early in life does their relationship between 5-HTTLPR and emotionality become apparent? In other words, how early does 5-HTTLPR start operating as a putative pathophysiologic mechanism and what would its potential utility be as an early biomarker? Considering that about 70% of mental illness has its origins in pre-adulthood, the potential importance of an answer to this question becomes particularly pressing. Some existing studies indicating that there is an early link between 5-HTTLPR and emotionality, other studies do not find a link. However, the small sample size of most of these studies does not allow for inferences to be drawn with a great degree of confidence. Here we propose to use a large, representative and well-characterized birth cohort, the ALSPAC dataset to study the link between 5-HTTLPR and emotionality in early life. Moreover, as we will detail in the research design that will help us overcome some of the typical challenges facing research into early emotionality, such as specificity of the findings and influence of reporter effects.

In summary, we propose to study a question that is central to current behavioural research-the genetic underpinnings of early human emotionality-and do so by using adequately-powered and rigorously controlled research methodology.

Work undertaken so far

1) Preliminary research within the ALSPAC cohort.

We have studied the instrument that we intend to use for measuring early emotionality, namely the well established Emotionality Activity Sociability (EAS) measure. As shown in Table 1, emotionality forms a distinct factor in the ALPAC population; it also shows good internal reliability. Moreover, as shown in Figure 1, we have demonstrated that emotionality measured at 38 months is an important predictor of internalising and some, but, crucially, not all forms of psychopathology in middle childhood (98 months).

2) Power analysis using the ALSPAC cohort.

Please see under "plan of investigation" for the relevant power analyses.

3) Published results

Our research efforts so far have focused on establishing the cross-sectional, three-year-, and 20-year follow-up associations of an important aspect of youth emotionality, irritability, in community samples.

In addition, we have established the prevalence and morbid associations of emotional instability (mood lability) in the general population.

* Brief plan of investigation, including methodology and design

Sample: The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children is a well-described birth cohort. DNA has been extracted and is available for 7000 mothers and 10000 children in the cohort.

Genotyping: Given previous reports about the 5-HTTLPR being functionally tri-allelic because of a substitution within the L allele, we will genotype the variable number tandem repeat (VNTR; short and long allele) as well as the single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP).

While the 5-HTTLPR has been partly genotyped before in ALSPAC, this was only done for a fraction of the children in the sample and the error rates were higher than what is commonly encountered in the literature. We have received advice on this matter and have established that the genotyping can be done at the Molecular Genetics Lab in the Institute of Psychiatry with a very high (less than 1%) precision rate (Professor Ian Craig, personal communication).

Phenotype: We will use the well-established (see above) EAS scale to measure emotionality and activity.

Main Analysis:

To test the main hypothesis that the low-expression genotype, an ANOVA with genotype as the categorical 3-level variable (low-, medium-, and high-expression) and human emotionality as the independent variable with planned post-hoc analysis to compare the low-expression genotype with the other two genotypes.

Controls:

1) Specificity of the finding: We will compare the results of emotionality with those of one of the other factors in the EAS for which we have established that it is a predictor of illness. This is in order to ensure that any the connection between 5-HTTLPR and emotionality is specific rather than a diffuse marker of pathology.

2) Impact of maternal genotype: It is well conceivable that maternal genotype influences ratings of emotionality either due to bias in reporting (mothers with a genotype linked to high emotionality may be biased towards scoring their children higher, for example) or an interaction between mothers and their children (mothers with a genotype linked to high emotionality may, for example, create an enviroment that is conducive to developing higher rates of emotionality). For this reason, we will conduct subanalyses stratifying for maternal genotype. For similar reasons, we will conduct subsidiary analyses stratifying for maternal history of mental illness.

Power Analysis:

We have used G-Power Version 3 (University of Dusseldorf) to calculate the power afforded by our sample to answer the main hypothesis and subsidiary questions of this study.

Assuming a sample size of 7000 children, an alpha level of 0.05 we would have a power of 100% to estimate an effect size as small as 0.1 in an F-test with a three-level categorical predictor variable. After stratifying for three maternal genotypes, essentially creating a 9-level variable, our power to detect a significant difference at 0.05 for an effect size as small as 0.1 would be 100%.

We will seek to confirm these estimates in our meeting with Dr Daniel Stahl, Department of Biostatistics, in a week's time.

Date proposal received: 
Tuesday, 28 July, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Tuesday, 28 July, 2009
Keywords: 
Genetics
Primary keyword: 

B851 - Does the contour card test predict visuoperceptual or reading difficulties in children in the ALSPAC cohort - 25/07/2009

B number: 
B851
Principal applicant name: 
Dr Feifei Shieh (University of Bristol, UK)
Co-applicants: 
Miss Cathy E M Williams (University of Bristol, UK), Dr Kate Northstone (University of Bristol, UK)
Title of project: 
Does the ?contour card? test predict visuoperceptual or reading difficulties in children in the ALSPAC cohort?
Proposal summary: 

ALSPAC provides an ideal opportunity for a pilot study to explore the extent to which the newly-designed "contour card" test indicates the integrity of any visuoperceptual functions in children aged 11-13 years. ALSPAC is an ongoing birth cohort study with a large amount of prospectively-collected data on visual functions and other abilities, in a contemporary sample of people who are now in their late teens. The contour cards were used in one of the ALSPAC clinics when the children were aged 11 years. An estimate of visuoperceptual skills in the children has been obtained by asking their mothers a battery of questions about their child's visuoperceptual abilities (based on Houliston et al., 1999). Both the raw visuoperceptual question responses, and the results of a factor analysis of these data, are being prepared for publication. We propose to compare the ALSPAC cohort members' performance in the contour card test at 11 with their visuoperceptual skills (as elicited in these questions), and their reading abilities, as theory and previous research predict that these might be linked with performance in the contour test. We would also include basic demographic data such as age and sex, and relevant eye problems such as strabismus and amblyopia. Outputs would be an MRes thesis to University of Bristol and if appropriate, a paper in a peer-reviewed journal.

Background

One of the oldest unsolved problems in vision science is how the visual system groups spatially separate elements of a display to form a coherent whole (May and Hess 2007). In vision science this is termed perceptual grouping, which refers to our ability to extract visual information from lower-level features to obtain perception of a higher-level structure. Psychophysical research has attempted to delineate principles by which local orientations and motions are combined across space to facilitate the detection of simple spatial contours. The perceived global structure of a scene depends heavily on the configuration and properties of its constituent local visual features (Ledgeway et al., 2005). Kovacs et al., 1999 developed a test of perceptual grouping, using "contour integration", that is suitable for use with paediatric patients in a clinical setting. This was motivated by research (1996) which found the detectability of a closed path (contour) defined by Gabor elements is significantly degraded in amblyopic eyes. These studies found that performance was degraded in strabismic, but not anisometropic amblyopia. Cells in the early parts of visual pathway have small receptive fields that are well tuned for orientation, direction of motion, and disparity (Norcia et al., 2005). Contours defined by Gabor patches cannot be detected by large filter mechanisms operating on the scale of the contour itself. Such elements force the observer to detect the contour on the basis of long-range comparisons between local orientation measurements at the defined spatial scale. The contour test has been devised to test perceptual grouping and has been shown to detect differences in people without ocular problems vs. those with amblyopia, as the above references mention. However, it has not been demonstrated whether the contour test can be useful as a measure of higher visual functions in children. We are interested to test children with problems in integrating different aspects of visual information (ie visuoperceptual problems), specifically those with "simultanagnosia". "Simultanagnosia" is a visuo-perceptual difficultly linked to object recognition in a crowded scene. This is thought to involve difficulties in using perceptual grouping to see the bigger picture made up from a number of component elements, thus "simultanagnosia" may be associated with poor performance in the contour test.

Details of the Contour detection card test

The cards consist of a closed chain of Gabor signals that roughly model the receptive field properties of orientation-selective simple cells in V1 (Kovacs et al., 1999). Contours defined by Gabor patches force the observer to detect the contour on the basis of long-range comparisons (Pennefather et al., 1998) for orientation differences of up to +/- 60(masculine ordinal indicator) amongst elements, which implies an association field and the path is reliably identified (Field et al., 1993; Loffer, 2008). Normal observers' performance on contour task starts to degrade at contrasts below 7-10% whilst amblyopes had thresholds outside of the 95% confidence interval range of normal observers (Kovacs et al., 2000). Contour integration deficits thus constitute an aspect of visual function that is disrupted by abnormal visual experience (Pennefather et al., 1999). (See below for an example of contour detection card).

The parameter (delta) is the ratio of background element spacing to contour element spacing. Variation of the parameter allows one to isolate first-order and second-order integration mechanisms, since the detection of the contour at values of (delta) less than 1 is impossible using first-order cues alone. The advantage of using (delta) parameter is that one can directly study the efficiency of long-range interactions that contribute to the integration of spatially distributed objects. By changing (delta), only the signal-to-noise ratio changes, while the shape of the contour, the global and local curvatures, the number of contour elements, the length of the contour, the spacing along the contour and the eccentricity of the elements are all kept constant. Contour integration deficits were found predominantly when the task required second-order information (ratio drops below 1) (Kovacs et al., 1999).

Few studies have been conducted to explore to what extent the contour test reflects day-to-day performance in particular tasks. However, adults with reading difficulties (diagnosed with dyslexia) have been reported to perform less well in the contour test than age-matched adults without reading problems (Simmers et al) .

Simultanagnosia

Simultanagnosia is a visual deficit described as a relative inability to comprehend more than one visual element in a scene and hence picture the whole. It has been described after prenatal damage to the brain, or after later-acquired parietal lobe injuries. We would like to test for covariation between results in the test of 'perceptual grouping' (the contour test) in children aged 11 and the reports of childrens' mothers on their abilities in the questions designed to elicit symptoms of visuoperceptual difficulties, including simultanagnosia. The questions asked in the ALSPAC questionnaire were originally devised to aid history-taking in patients with hydrocephalus, who often have vision and visuoperceptual problems. Questions about visuoperceptual problems have not been widely used in samples of normally developing children. The ALSPAC mothers' responses to the questions have been analysed looking for common underlying themes, using principal components analysis (PCA) and the data suggest that the mothers' responses cluster around 3 "factors": Factor 1 correlates with questions about seeing objects surrounded by clutter (similar to simultanagnosia as described above); Factor 2 correlates with visuomotor problems (difficulties making judgements about positions of self or objects within 3D space), and Factor 3 with questions about recognition of faces. The first two are thought to be mediated primarily by a part of the brain known as "the dorsal pathway" and the latter by another area called "the ventral pathway". These hypothesized functional sub-divisions within the brain are based on experimental evidence and on observations in clinical literature: the dorsal pathway is thought to carry information about where objects are in space, whilst the ventral pathway is more responsible for fine detail and recognition.

Objective

We would like to test the childrens' raw visuoperceptual scores as reported by the mothers, and 3 PCA-derived factors of visuoperceptual skills, in comparison with the contour test results. We will take account of other potentially important variables such as visual acuity (at appropriate age) and factors known to impair performance in the contour test eg strabismus and amblyopia. We would also like to look at a measure of reading ability with respect to the contour results, to estimate whether the contour test will usefully predict reading difficulties in this cohort. We hypothesize that the contour test results will show stronger associations with Factor 1 than with Factors 2 or 3. We also hypothesize that some children with reading difficulties (ie reading worse than that predicted by their IQ) will perform less well in the contour test than will children with reading skills commensurate with their IQ.

We therefore request access to these specific variables:

* Child's sex

* Mother's highest educational attainment

* Parent's socio-economic status (SES from pregnancy questionnaires)

* Child's reading ability at 13 (TOWRE test at tf2)

* Childs IQ (WISC at F8 clinic)

* Child's visual status (strabismus and/or amblyopia assessed at F7 clinic)

* Child's contour test results (F11 clinic)

* Child's visuoperceptual skills (questions asked of mum in 13-yr child-based questionnaire).

The analytic strategy will start with tests of correlation, either parametric or non-parametric depending on the distribution of the data. We will then use Logistic regression and/or Generalised Linear Modelling (GLM) to obtain estimates for the associations between the contour test results and either reading or visuoperceptual skills, adjusted for relevant potential confounders.

References

Field, D.J., Hayes, A., Hess, R.F. (1993) Contour integration by human visual system: evidence for a "local association field" Vision Res, 33, 173-93

Houliston, M.J., Taguri, A.H., Dutton, G.N., Hajivssiliou, C., Young, D.G. (1999) Evidence of cognitive visual problems in children with hydrocephalus: a structured clinical history-taking strategy. Dev Med Neurol, 41, 298-306

Kovacs, I., Kozma, P., Feher, A., Benedek, G. (1999) Late maturation of visual spatial integration in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 96, 12204-9

Ledgeway, T., Hess, R.F., and Geisler, W.S. (2005). Grouping local orientation and direction signals to extract spatial contours. Empirical tests of "association field" models of contour integration. Vision Research, 45, 2511-2522

Loffler, G. (2008) 'Perception of contours and shapes: Low and intermediate stage mechanisms', Vision Research, 48, 2106-27

May, K.A. And Hess, R.F. (2007). Dynamics of snakes and ladders. Journal of Vision, 7, 1-9

Norcia, A.M., Sampath, V., Hou, C. (2005). Experience-expectant development of contour integration mechanisms in human visual cortex. Journal of Vision. 5, 116-130

Pennefather, P.M., Chandna, A., Kovacs, I., Polat, U., and Norcia, A.M., (1999) Contour detection threshold: repeatability and learning with 'contour cards'. Spatial Vision 12, 257-266

Simmer and Bex. Deficits in Visual Contour Integration in Dyslexia. IOVS 2001 42: 2737 - 2742.

Date proposal received: 
Saturday, 25 July, 2009
Date proposal approved: 
Saturday, 25 July, 2009
Keywords: 
Vision
Primary keyword: 

Pages